2013
DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2013.02.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Conceptual influences on category-based induction

Abstract: One important function of categories is to permit rich inductive inferences. Prior work shows that children use category labels to guide their inductive inferences. However, there are competing theories to explain this phenomenon, differing in the roles attributed to conceptual information versus perceptual similarity. Seven experiments with 4- to 5-year-old children and adults (N = 344) test these theories by teaching categories for which category membership and perceptual similarity are in conflict, and vary… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
80
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 72 publications
(88 citation statements)
references
References 53 publications
(70 reference statements)
1
80
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Nonetheless, young children tend to spontaneously focus on highly salient surface features. Specifically, while older children and adults often group objects according to complex cues such as common internal properties, labels, and causal affordances, regardless of perceptual similarity (Carey, 1985;Keil, 1989;Medin, 1989;Rips, 1989), young children tend to group objects based on perceptual similarity, and only later shift to favoring other properties (e.g., Gelman & Davidson, 2013;Gentner, 2010;Keil & Batterman, 1984).…”
Section: Inductive Generalization: a Shift From Perceptual To Conceptmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Nonetheless, young children tend to spontaneously focus on highly salient surface features. Specifically, while older children and adults often group objects according to complex cues such as common internal properties, labels, and causal affordances, regardless of perceptual similarity (Carey, 1985;Keil, 1989;Medin, 1989;Rips, 1989), young children tend to group objects based on perceptual similarity, and only later shift to favoring other properties (e.g., Gelman & Davidson, 2013;Gentner, 2010;Keil & Batterman, 1984).…”
Section: Inductive Generalization: a Shift From Perceptual To Conceptmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It should be noted that debates about the mechanisms of inductive generalization often are framed in terms of perception versus knowledge (Booth & Waxman, 2002;Gelman & Davidson, 2013;Gelman & Markman, 1986;Sloutsky & Fisher, 2004;Sloutsky, Kloos, & Fisher, 2007). However, the juxtaposition of perception and knowledge is a false dichotomy (for similar arguments see Smith, Jones, Yoshida, & Colunga, 2003).…”
Section: Perceptual and Representational Similarity (Pars) Account Ofmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although category membership can be inferred without language (e.g., recognizing an animal as a snake based on its shape and movement), labels have informational capacity beyond direct observation. Even for young children, they can convey surprising category membership of an individual item (e.g., that a legless lizard is not a snake) (31) or introduce wholly new categorical distinctions (e.g., to distinguish animals based on subtle variations in antennae rather than overall body shape) (32).…”
Section: Categories As Cultural Inheritancementioning
confidence: 99%