2004
DOI: 10.1590/s0001-37652004000200013
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Does the environment constrain avian sound localization?

Abstract: A bird needs to keep track not only of social interactions of conspecifics but also of their changing locations in space by determining their directions and distances. Current knowledge of accuracy in the computation of sound source location by birds is still insufficient, partly because physiological mechanisms of few species are studied in well defined laboratory settings, while field studies are performed in a variety of species and complex environments. Velocity gradients and reverberating surfaces may con… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
9
0

Year Published

2004
2004
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 9 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 22 publications
0
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…These results are interesting because 'seet' calls are produced with relatively high sound frequencies (~7-8·kHz), leading to the suggestion that they might be difficult to localize (e.g. Larsen, 2004;Marler, 1955).…”
Section: Experiments I: Azimuth Judgments In the Fieldmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…These results are interesting because 'seet' calls are produced with relatively high sound frequencies (~7-8·kHz), leading to the suggestion that they might be difficult to localize (e.g. Larsen, 2004;Marler, 1955).…”
Section: Experiments I: Azimuth Judgments In the Fieldmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Calford and Piddington, 1988;Hyson et al, 1994). We do not yet fully understand how the interaural pathway functions in small birds (Klump, 2000;Larsen, 2004;Larsen et al, 1997), however female parasitoid flies can assess azimuth with a resolution of only 2° (Mason et al, 2001) and these flies appear to use mechanical coupling between the left and right tympani to achieve this high level of performance (e.g. Mason et al, 2001;Miles et al, 1995;Robert et al, 1996).…”
Section: Overall Performancementioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The maximum ILD is about 10 dB for an 8 kHz tone presented slightly behind the animal (e.g., Klump, 2000), and is 5.6 dB for a 4 kHz tone presented off to the side (Larsen et al, 2006). However, a maximum difference of around 3 dB has been measured for frequencies below 4 kHz, the range where budgerigars hear best (Dooling et al, 2000;Park and Dooling, 1991;Larsen, 2004;Larsen et al, 2006). Park and Dooling (1991) computed the minimum ITDs for budgerigars based on their interaural distance and minimum audible angles, and found that acuity would be around 50 ls, at best (e.g., for 2 kHz stimuli).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Whether or not behavioral thresholds match these predictions remains to be seen. Small birds, like mammals, are thought to use both ITDs and ILDs to localize sounds (Klump, 2000;Larsen, 2004;Nelson and Suthers, 2004), though no lateralization tasks have been conducted to explore this possibility. It has been suggested that great tits (Parus major), birds with a similar interaural distance to the budgerigar, for example, might be resolving binaural time differences of about 18 ls to account for their free field acuity of 20 (Klump et al, 1986).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%