2016
DOI: 10.1590/0004-282x20160070
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Impaired executive functions in experimental model of temporal lobe epilepsy

Abstract: with approximately 65 million people affected worldwide, epilepsy is one of the most common, chronic and serious neurological disease 1,2,3,4 . temporal lobe epilepsy (tLE) is the commonest form of human epilepsy, affecting approximately 40% of patients and often resistant to antiepileptic drugs 5,6,7 . From the structural and functional points of view, tLE is often associated with specific structural abnormalities that affect limbic structures as well as frontal lobe, which is associated with cognitive proces… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

4
1
0
1

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

2
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 11 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
4
1
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…In addition, we found that the swimming speed was significantly increased in experimental rats (data not shown). This result is consistent with previous studies showing that rats with pilocarpine‐induced epilepsy exhibit attention deficit hyperactivity disorder‐like behaviours, such as increased locomotor activity and impulsivity [33, 34]. Together, these cognitive and behavioural abnormalities likely contribute to the impaired performance of epileptic rats in the MWM task.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…In addition, we found that the swimming speed was significantly increased in experimental rats (data not shown). This result is consistent with previous studies showing that rats with pilocarpine‐induced epilepsy exhibit attention deficit hyperactivity disorder‐like behaviours, such as increased locomotor activity and impulsivity [33, 34]. Together, these cognitive and behavioural abnormalities likely contribute to the impaired performance of epileptic rats in the MWM task.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…Thus, animals, although likely now less fearful, spent more time in the periphery exploring the holes, as evidenced by the number of nose-pokes (9.0 ± 0.7, n = 16). SRS animals behaved similarly when exposed to the holeboard, since the total number of rearings (8.6 ± 1.0, n = 16) was only a bit higher for SRS animals than the one performed by Sham animals, while the number of nose-pokes was slightly lower (7.8 ± 1.2, n = 16), yet the total distance travelled by SRS animals (26.1 ± 2.7 m, n = 16) in the holeboard was double the one travelled by Sham animals (13.0 ± 1.4 m, n = 16), as occurring in virtually all other tests, a reflection of their anxious and attention deficit-like behaviour, as previously described [37,38].…”
Section: Mismatch Novelty Detectionsupporting
confidence: 75%
“…Thus, animals although likely now less fearful, spent more time in the periphery exploring the holes, as evidenced by the number of nose-pokes (9.0±0.7, n=16). SRSs animals behaved similarly when exposed to the holeboard, since the total number of rearings (8.6±1.0, n=16) was only a bit higher for SRSs animals than the one performed by Sham animals, while the number of nose-pokes was slightly lower (7.8±1.2, n=16), yet the total distance travelled by SRSs animals (26.1±2.7m, n=16) in the holeboard was double the one travelled by Sham animals (13.0±1.4m, n=16), as occurring in virtually all other tests, a reflection of their anxious and attention deficit -like behaviour, as previously described [35,36].…”
Section: Mismatch Novelty Detectionsupporting
confidence: 73%