1999
DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.25.4.986
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

What do misestimations and asymmetries in spatial judgement indicate about spatial representation?

Abstract: When people are asked to judge the distance between 2 points, they may produce systematic over-or underestimations. Their judgments may also show asymmetries, as when, for example, people estimate the distance from their house to a mailbox as different from the distance from the mailbox to their house. It has been argued that such errors show that spatial representations are fundamentally nonmetric. In 3 experiments, however, the authors show that these effects can be explained by a category-adjustment (CA) mo… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2

Citation Types

6
52
1
1

Year Published

2002
2002
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 59 publications
(61 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
6
52
1
1
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, using the mental scanning paradigm with described targets, Denis and Cocude (1997) were unable to find any spatial biases when scanning toward salient as compared with toward neutral landmarks. Asymmetries in spatial responses directed toward salient and nonsalient locations are a typical finding with real-world environments (McNamara & Diwadkar, 1997;Newcombe, Huttenlocher, Sandberg, Lie, & Johnson, 1999). Moreover, spatial representations derived from language do not seem to be as vivid as ones derived from perception.…”
mentioning
confidence: 97%
“…For example, using the mental scanning paradigm with described targets, Denis and Cocude (1997) were unable to find any spatial biases when scanning toward salient as compared with toward neutral landmarks. Asymmetries in spatial responses directed toward salient and nonsalient locations are a typical finding with real-world environments (McNamara & Diwadkar, 1997;Newcombe, Huttenlocher, Sandberg, Lie, & Johnson, 1999). Moreover, spatial representations derived from language do not seem to be as vivid as ones derived from perception.…”
mentioning
confidence: 97%
“…For instance, people weight prototypical information more heavily when fine-grained information is uncertain, such as when multiple locations must be remembered. Engebretson and Huttenlocher (1996) found larger response biases toward the centers of the left and right regions in the V task when participants were asked to remember two target directions presented sequentially than when a single target had to be remembered (see also Huttenlocher et al, 1991;Newcombe et al, 1999).…”
Section: Spatial Categories and The Ca Modelmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Huttenlocher and colleagues have proposed that similar categorization processes operate when people make a variety of stimulus judgments, including judgments of object size , estimates of location from memory (Engebretson & Huttenlocher, 1996;Huttenlocher et al, 1991;Newcombe et al, 1999), judgments involving event timing and duration (Huttenlocher et al, 1988), and even estimates of grayness . This proposal has been formalized in the CA model, which we introduce in the context of location memory tasks, the focus of the present study.…”
Section: Spatial Categories and The Ca Modelmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Specifically, we looked at performance on distance estimation and direction judgment tasks as a function of the need for fine-and coarse-grained information. This was done from the perspective of Huttenlocher, Hedges, and Duncan's (1991;Newcombe, Huttenlocher, Sandberg, Lie, & Johnson, 1999) category adjustment theory of spatial memory.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%