2000
DOI: 10.1111/1467-9280.00270
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Visual Capture of Touch: Out-of-the-Body Experiences With Rubber Gloves

Abstract: When the apparent visual location of a body part conflicts with its veridical location, vision can dominate proprioception and kinesthesia. In this article, we show that vision can capture tactile localization. Participants discriminated the location of vibrotactile stimuli (upper, at the index finger, vs. lower, at the thumb), while ignoring distractor lights that could independently be upper or lower. Such tactile discriminations were slowed when the distractor light was incongruent with the tactile target (… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

50
451
1
1

Year Published

2003
2003
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
4

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 527 publications
(503 citation statements)
references
References 21 publications
50
451
1
1
Order By: Relevance
“…As a result, the visual information regarding the fake hand location, rather than the real hand location, combined with the visual information was able to elicit the facilitated target detection. Thus, the present findings are consistent with studies such as Pavani et al (2000). Pavani et al used a paradigm in which the visual location of a rubber hand competed with the location of a hidden, real hand.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…As a result, the visual information regarding the fake hand location, rather than the real hand location, combined with the visual information was able to elicit the facilitated target detection. Thus, the present findings are consistent with studies such as Pavani et al (2000). Pavani et al used a paradigm in which the visual location of a rubber hand competed with the location of a hidden, real hand.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…This is consistent with the claim of that synchronous stroking is a necessary prerequisite of the RHI. This hypothesis is at odds, however, with the findings of at least two studies that have reported significant effects of seeing a rubber hand without any stroking whatsoever (Farnè, Pavani, Meneghello, & Làdavas, 2000;Pavani, Spence, & Driver, 2000). We suggest that there may be (at least) two independent causes of the RHI: one driven purely by visual perception of the rubber hand in the proper position and orientation, another driven by the synchronous stroking of the participants' and the rubber hand.…”
Section: Subjective Reports Of Rubber Hand Illusionmentioning
confidence: 59%
“…If this were the case, signals from any external object could potentially be integrated with bodily-related signals inducing a sense of ownership for that object. However, this does not normally occur, since the RHI is not induced (or it is much weaker) when visuo-tactile stimulation is applied to a non-corporeal object (Armel & Ramachandran, 2003;Haans, Ijsselsteijn, & de Kort, 2008;Tsakiris, Carpenter, James, & Fotopoulou, 2010;Tsakiris & Haggard, 2005) or when the rubber hand is placed in a position that is anatomically incompatible with the subject's posture (Costantini & Haggard, 2007;Lloyd, 2007;Pavani, Spence, & Driver, 2000). Tsakiris and Haggard (2005) and more recently Tsakiris (2010) argued that the sense of body ownership also depends on top-down processes, which act over and above multisensory integration mechanisms and help distinguish between objects that can and cannot be attributed to one's body as a function of their visual appearance and anatomical coherence with the physical body.…”
Section: Over and Above Multisensory Integrationmentioning
confidence: 99%