Space and Time in Perception and Action 2010
DOI: 10.1017/cbo9780511750540.004
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Visual and nonvisual factors in perisaccadic compression of space

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2010
2010
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
4

Relationship

0
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 34 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Indeed, when visual references are available, a stimulus flashed spatially before the saccade target is displaced in the direction of the saccade whereas a bar flashed spatially beyond the target is displaced in the opposite direction (e.g., Lappe, Awater, & Krekelberg, 2000;. This peri-saccadic compression has been investigated by manipulating visual characteristics of stimuli such as contrast (Michels & Lappe, 2004), color (Lappe, Kuhlmann, Oerke, & Kaiser, 2006), luminance (Georg, Hamker, & Lappe, 2008), or by manipulating the modality of the localization report: verbal reporting of the position or the number of bars (Awater, Burr, Lappe, Morrone, & Goldberg, 2005;Matsumiya & Uchikawa, 2001;, manual pointing (Bruno & Morrone, 2007), mouse pointing (Lappe et al, 2000;Ostendorf, Fischer, Finke, & Ploner, 2007;Reeve, Clark, & O'Regan, 2008), or saccade targeting (Awater & Lappe, 2006;Lappe, Michels, & Awater, 2009).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Indeed, when visual references are available, a stimulus flashed spatially before the saccade target is displaced in the direction of the saccade whereas a bar flashed spatially beyond the target is displaced in the opposite direction (e.g., Lappe, Awater, & Krekelberg, 2000;. This peri-saccadic compression has been investigated by manipulating visual characteristics of stimuli such as contrast (Michels & Lappe, 2004), color (Lappe, Kuhlmann, Oerke, & Kaiser, 2006), luminance (Georg, Hamker, & Lappe, 2008), or by manipulating the modality of the localization report: verbal reporting of the position or the number of bars (Awater, Burr, Lappe, Morrone, & Goldberg, 2005;Matsumiya & Uchikawa, 2001;, manual pointing (Bruno & Morrone, 2007), mouse pointing (Lappe et al, 2000;Ostendorf, Fischer, Finke, & Ploner, 2007;Reeve, Clark, & O'Regan, 2008), or saccade targeting (Awater & Lappe, 2006;Lappe, Michels, & Awater, 2009).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the current experiments, the reference bar acts as a strong visual attractor. For saccadic compression, it has been suggested that attraction is dependent on extra-retinal signals: corollary discharge or predictive eye position information that build up strong modulatory signals around the endpoint of the saccade (e.g., Morrone et al, 1997 ; Lappe et al, 2010 ). Accordingly, compression was found to be centered on locations in empty space in saccadic adaptation and anti-saccade paradigms (Awater and Lappe, 2004 ; Awater et al, 2005 ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It has been suggested that compression reflects an underestimation of relative distance rather than a mislocalization of the absolute position of the probe (Lappe et al, 2010 ). Along similar lines, we recently proposed that compression may reflect an underestimation of distance due to a lack of perceived apparent motion seen between the reference and the subsequently presented probe (Zimmermann et al, 2014a ; Born et al, 2015 ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation