2021
DOI: 10.1101/2021.07.21.453253
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Transfer of learned cognitive flexibility to novel stimuli and task sets

Abstract: Adaptive behavior requires learning about the structure of the environment to derive optimal action policies, and previous studies have documented transfer of such structural knowledge to bias choices in new environments. Here, we asked whether people could also acquire and transfer more abstract knowledge across different task environments, in particular, expectations about demands on cognitive control. Over three experiments, participants performed a probabilistic card-sorting task in environments of either … Show more

Help me understand this report
View published versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
3
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
2

Relationship

0
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 2 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 51 publications
1
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This is consistent with several previous studies showing that people seem to weigh unexpected positive versus negative outcomes separately when updating action values (Gershman, 2015;Niv et al, 2012;& Lebreton, 2022;Rosenbaum et al, 2022). Perhaps most noteworthy, this is also in line with another recent study that investigated inertia of used learning rates into a separate subsequent transfer phase (Wen et al, 2023). Specifically, they manipulated volatilities between subjects (i.e., each subject was given one volatility level) in a Wisconsin Card Sorting Task (WCST).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…This is consistent with several previous studies showing that people seem to weigh unexpected positive versus negative outcomes separately when updating action values (Gershman, 2015;Niv et al, 2012;& Lebreton, 2022;Rosenbaum et al, 2022). Perhaps most noteworthy, this is also in line with another recent study that investigated inertia of used learning rates into a separate subsequent transfer phase (Wen et al, 2023). Specifically, they manipulated volatilities between subjects (i.e., each subject was given one volatility level) in a Wisconsin Card Sorting Task (WCST).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…Most importantly, we compared models with either a shared learning rate across both environments, versus models with different, environment-specific learning rates for both casinos. Furthermore, we compared models with separate learning rates after positive and negative reward feedback (Gershman, 2015;Niv et al, 2012;Palminteri & Lebreton, 2022;Rosenbaum et al, 2022, Wen et al, 2023, to models with one learning rate for both feedback types. To quantify the alternative hypothesis that learning rate changes merely reflect local adaptations to the experienced prediction errors, we also implemented models with variable learning rates that depend on recently experienced prediction errors (Bai et al, 2014;Krugel et al, 2009;Li et al, 2011;Pearce & Hall, 1980).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…While a stable context is considered a necessary condition for habits to form and be expressed [3][4][5] , the influence of the learned higher-order properties of the task context on habit learning and expression have not yet been studied. Computational and behavioural work on cognitive flexibility shows that humans engage in such meta-learning, and use this knowledge to optimize performance [6][7][8][9][10] . For example, environments where S-R-O contingencies frequently change encourage higher learning rates than more stable contexts 6 .…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%