2017
DOI: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010416-044242
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Toward a Social Psychophysics of Face Communication

Abstract: As a highly social species, humans are equipped with a powerful tool for social communication-the face. Although seemingly simple, the human face can elicit multiple social perceptions due to the rich variations of its movements, morphology, and complexion. Consequently, identifying precisely what face information elicits different social perceptions is a complex empirical challenge that has largely remained beyond the reach of traditional methods. In the past decade, the emerging field of social psychophysics… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
135
0
2

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 145 publications
(138 citation statements)
references
References 115 publications
(82 reference statements)
1
135
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…In Cowen and Keltner’s study, however, each participant viewed a subsample of the available movie clips (30 of the 2185 available), allowing the possibility of treating clips as a random-effects variable. Cowen and Keltner did not explicitly estimate stimulus variability and adjust for uncertainty due to stimulus sampling, but future studies can (for an example of modeling stimuli as a random effect, see [3]). Also of note, the analyses were conducted on the average of the individual ratings associated with each film clip, rather than on the individual ratings themselves.…”
Section: Study Designed For Robust and Replicable Findingsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In Cowen and Keltner’s study, however, each participant viewed a subsample of the available movie clips (30 of the 2185 available), allowing the possibility of treating clips as a random-effects variable. Cowen and Keltner did not explicitly estimate stimulus variability and adjust for uncertainty due to stimulus sampling, but future studies can (for an example of modeling stimuli as a random effect, see [3]). Also of note, the analyses were conducted on the average of the individual ratings associated with each film clip, rather than on the individual ratings themselves.…”
Section: Study Designed For Robust and Replicable Findingsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Reverse correlation methods have proven their usefulness in social perception research as a data-driven tool to probe a perceiver's a priori expectations (or internal representations) about their social world (for a review see Brinkman, Todorov, & Dotsch, 2017;Jack & Schyns, 2017). These methods allow the identification of face configurations that are diagnostic of specific social judgments.…”
Section: Reverse Correlation Methodologymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The noise generation procedure was identical to the one reported in Dotsch and Todorov (2012). The randomness of the task stimuli circumvents a priori assumptions made about the facial content associated with any of the target trait dimensions (Gosselin & Schyns, 2003;Jack & Schyns, 2017). The base image was the grayscale average male face (Karolinska Face Database; Lundqvist, Flykt, & Ohman, 1998), resized to 256 9 256 pixels (Dotsch & Todorov, 2012).…”
Section: Face Stimulimentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This culture priming approach was recently employed to demonstrate that the impact of “group” facial actions on emotion perception in a target individual, previously documented in East Asians, can be produced by priming a collectivistic cultural value orientation (compared to an individualistic orientation) in Greek participants [51]. Similarly, research that reveals the internal perceptual representations of emotions via psychophysical methods [52] also underscores how culture, in-the-head, shapes emotion perception [Jack, this issue].…”
Section: Refining Our Approach To Culturementioning
confidence: 99%