2010
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.016
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The shape of words in the brain

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

12
146
2

Year Published

2011
2011
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 136 publications
(164 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
12
146
2
Order By: Relevance
“…For words that feature early in language acquisition, systematicity is prominent, but for later-acquired words, the form-meaning mappings reveal increasing arbitrariness. The enhanced systematicity for the early vocabulary supports views that systematicity is useful for language acquisition [15][16][17][18]20]. Systematicity promotes understanding of the communicative function of language early in development, as the form provides information to the learner about the meaning, potentially enabling the child to learn that words have referents.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 70%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For words that feature early in language acquisition, systematicity is prominent, but for later-acquired words, the form-meaning mappings reveal increasing arbitrariness. The enhanced systematicity for the early vocabulary supports views that systematicity is useful for language acquisition [15][16][17][18]20]. Systematicity promotes understanding of the communicative function of language early in development, as the form provides information to the learner about the meaning, potentially enabling the child to learn that words have referents.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 70%
“…For example, in English, words associated with the nose and its functions tend to begin with sn-, or words referring to light often begin with gl- [6]. Preferences for certain sound-meaning relationships, have been demonstrated to affect learning of novel adjectives [15], verbs [16,17], nouns [18,19] and mixes thereof [20], though these studies generally test a forced choice between two alternatives. When the semantic distinction is not immediately available, as in a forced-choice test between two objects from different categories, then learning is less evident but still present under some learning conditions [21].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Now, while Parise and Spence's (2009) results demonstrated increased spatial and temporal integration for pairs of auditory and visual stimuli that share a crossmodal correspondence, other researchers have argued that the perceptual salience of crossmodally congruent (as compared to incongruent) pairs of auditory and visual stimuli may also be heightened (see, e.g., Evans & Treisman, 2010;Marks, 1987a; though see Marks et al, 2003;Melara, 1989b). Indeed, the latest electrophysiological research has demonstrated that when crossmodally matching (as compared to mismatching) pairs of stimuli are presented, the early neural evoked response (e.g., N1) may peak significantly earlier and have an enhanced amplitude (e.g., Kovic, Plunkett, & Westermann, 2009; see also Seo et al, 2010). Results such as these have been taken to support a perceptual enhancement account of at least some part of certain crossmodal correspondence effects.…”
Section: Interim Summarymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…bouba and kiki judged to correspond to a round, curvy versus jagged, pointy shape, respectively [20][21][22]. Iconic mappings have also been shown to be facilitatory in studies using indirect measures of online processing, including reaction times [23,24] and EEG waveforms [25,26], as well as to facilitate language acquisition in both children and adults [27,28]. Finally, prosody, or the suprasegmental modulation of the acoustic signal, constitutes another channel of expression in which iconic mappings may be expressed.…”
Section: Language Studies: the Current Focus Approaches And Assumptionsmentioning
confidence: 99%