2018
DOI: 10.1101/328369
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The relationship between subjective sleep quality and cognitive performance in healthy young adults: Evidence from three empirical studies

Abstract: Background:The role of sleep in cognitive performance has gained increasing attention in neuroscience and sleep research in the recent decades, however, the relationship between subjective (self-reported) sleep quality and cognitive performance has not yet been comprehensively characterized. In this paper, our aim was to test the relationship between subjective sleep quality and a wide range of cognitive functions in healthy young adults across three studies. Sleep quality was assessed by Pittsburgh Sleep Qual… Show more

Help me understand this report
View published versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

0
6
1

Year Published

2020
2020
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 58 publications
(40 reference statements)
0
6
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The authors also noted that age associated positively with sleep duration and negatively with sleep efficiency 15 , indicating that younger participants likely slept longer than 8.2 ± 0.6 h/night with an efficiency below 86.5 ± 5.1%, the reported averages for each parameter. A recent study of healthy adults with a mean age closer to that of our participants (approximately 21 years) found no association between subjective sleep quality and working memory 38 . The authors suggest that the lack of association may have been due to a ceiling effect of studying a healthy population with limited prevalence of disorders known to cause sleep disturbances, which is also the case in the present study.…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 61%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The authors also noted that age associated positively with sleep duration and negatively with sleep efficiency 15 , indicating that younger participants likely slept longer than 8.2 ± 0.6 h/night with an efficiency below 86.5 ± 5.1%, the reported averages for each parameter. A recent study of healthy adults with a mean age closer to that of our participants (approximately 21 years) found no association between subjective sleep quality and working memory 38 . The authors suggest that the lack of association may have been due to a ceiling effect of studying a healthy population with limited prevalence of disorders known to cause sleep disturbances, which is also the case in the present study.…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 61%
“…On the other hand, the minimal associations between free-living sleep and cognitive function may have been due to a ceiling effect in the performance on the cognitive tasks related to the peak in the cognitive function that www.nature.com/scientificreports/ reportedly occurs in young adulthood, close to the age range of our generally healthy, older adolescent population. Working memory has been reported to peak in young adulthood 38,41,46,47 . In support of this, the responses on all cognitive loads of the working memory task in the current study were faster and more accurate than in a previous study of male participants with a slightly older mean age (28 ± 4 years) tested with the same paradigm in our laboratory 48 .…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Sleep is important to physical, cognitive, and psychological health. Sleep quality changes as a function of normal aging, there is no association between subjective sleep quality and cognitive performance in healthy young adults [10], and only half of middle-aged and elderly Chinese reported good sleep quality [11]. Agerelated sleep changes may lead to poor sleep quality in older adults with physical or psychiatric disorders [12].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Bastien et al, 2014 for a review) and healthy populations, namely in the elderly, who often report a relatively good perception of sleep despite marked objective sleep quality disruptions (Buysse et al, 1991;Vitiello et al, 2004;Zilli et al, 2009). It has been argued that objective and subjective sleep assessments may not necessarily measure the same aspects of sleep quality and disturbance and actually rely on different parameters (Goelema et al, 2019;Zavecz et al, 2018). Another possible explanation of the mixed and discrepant data is that traditional sleep measures, such as SE, WASO time, total sleep duration and sleep stage proportions, while useful to describe overall sleep macrostructure, could be insufficient to detect subtler disruptions of the sleep episode.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%