1980
DOI: 10.1177/154079698000500204
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Generalized Effects of Productive Labeling Training Involving Common Object Classes

Abstract: Initially, a mentally retarded subject demonstrated, by matching-to-sample, appropriate classification of common stimuli (i.e., cars, bowls, hats, dolls, shoes, and books). Next he was taught to produce the correct verbal label (e.g., ''bowl'') for one member of each class. For four of the six stimulus classes, a single training example was not sufficient to produce generalization of the label to nontraining examples in the same class. However, each successive addition of one training example generally produce… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
6
0

Year Published

1988
1988
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 10 publications
0
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Because an important educational objective is to establish a flexible, bidirectional verbal repertoire (Hayes, 1991;, it would be important to know how receptive and expressive skills can become functionally related to one another. Ample data suggest that such skills may on occasion function independently (e.g., Anderson & Spradlin, 1980;Guess & Baer, 1973; and see reviews by Goldstein, 1993;Stromer & Mackay, in press).…”
Section: Matching To Sample and Class Formationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Because an important educational objective is to establish a flexible, bidirectional verbal repertoire (Hayes, 1991;, it would be important to know how receptive and expressive skills can become functionally related to one another. Ample data suggest that such skills may on occasion function independently (e.g., Anderson & Spradlin, 1980;Guess & Baer, 1973; and see reviews by Goldstein, 1993;Stromer & Mackay, in press).…”
Section: Matching To Sample and Class Formationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…under a variety of conditions (i.e., in different class periods and times; with different staff, students) and to perform variations of the targeted social skills, such as sharing (e.g., while eating lunch, using playground equipment, working with materials in a group), which are the intended outcomes of stimulus and response generalization (Albin & Horner, 1988;Anderson & Spradlin, 1980;Dunlap, Koegel, & Koegel, 1984;Horner, McDonnell, & Bellamy, 1986;Horner, Sprague, & Wilcox, 1982;Sprague & Horner, 1984;Stokes & Baer, 1977) Fifth, enabling students to respond to natural cues and contingencies is critical to maximize generalization of acquired skills in target environments (Stokes & Baer, 1977). School and community settings are rich with naturally occurring social opportunities.…”
Section: Evolution Of Key Elements For Maximizing Instructional Outcomesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, the method of sufficient exemplars was explicitly described as a more systematic introduction of reinforcement and as a method involving repeated presentation of new exemplars. These new exemplars would probably constitute a stimulus class desired for control (e.g., Anderson & Spradlin, 1980;Stokes et al, 1974), although this was not a condition Stokes and Baer specified. In instances where insufficient generalization occurs because extraneous stimuli have come to control behavior, this tactic may lead to elimination ofthe extraneous control (e.g., Garcia, 1974;Griffiths & Craighead, 1972).…”
Section: Sequential Modification and Train Sufficient Exemplarsmentioning
confidence: 99%