2017
DOI: 10.1177/0145445517742883
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Systematic Changes in Preference for Schedule-Thinning Arrangements as a Function of Relative Reinforcement Density

Abstract: We treated destructive behavior maintained by both social-positive (i.e., access to tangibles) and social-negative (i.e., escape from demands) reinforcement in an individual diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder using functional communication training (FCT). We then thinned the schedule of reinforcement for the tangible function using a multiple schedule (mult FCT) and later thinned the availability of escape using a chained schedule (chain FCT). Both treatments proved effective at maintaining functional com… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

0
22
1

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
10

Relationship

5
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 24 publications
(23 citation statements)
references
References 44 publications
0
22
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Other potential factors influencing outcomes could include preference for receiving reinforcers spaced over time, the increased value of a reinforcer following the effort of completing a response requirement (Hackenberg, 2009), or the density of reinforcement in a given condition if proper yoking procedures were not employed (Briggs et al, 2018;Herrnstein, 1961). Although the outcomes from the statistical analyses did not suggest that yoking procedures made a significant difference in preference outcomes, this inclusion of studies that used no yoking procedures likely affected this.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Other potential factors influencing outcomes could include preference for receiving reinforcers spaced over time, the increased value of a reinforcer following the effort of completing a response requirement (Hackenberg, 2009), or the density of reinforcement in a given condition if proper yoking procedures were not employed (Briggs et al, 2018;Herrnstein, 1961). Although the outcomes from the statistical analyses did not suggest that yoking procedures made a significant difference in preference outcomes, this inclusion of studies that used no yoking procedures likely affected this.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Case 3 later participated as Samantha in Briggs, Akers, Greer, Fisher, and Retzlaff (). None of her data overlap across the two studies.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Behavior analysts generally select mult FCT for destructive behavior maintained by social positive reinforcement (e.g., attention, tangible items) by alternating two schedules of reinforcementā€”a reinforcement component (signaled by the S D ) in which the behavior analyst reinforces the FCR according to a fixed-ratio (FR) 1 and an extinction component (signaled by the S Ī” ) in which the reinforcer is unavailable (for a review, see Saini, Miller, & Fisher, 2016). By developing discriminative control over the FCR (i.e., FCRs occurring exclusively in the S D component), behavior analysts have used mult FCT to thin reinforcement schedules for the FCR gradually (e.g., Greer et al, 2016; Hanley et al, 2001; Jarmolowicz, DeLeon, & Kuhn, 2009; Rooker et al, 2013) and rapidly (e.g., Betz et al, 2013; Briggs, Akers, Greer, Fisher, & Retzlaff, 2018; Fisher, Greer, Fuhrman, & Querim, 2015; Fuhrman, Fisher, & Greer, 2016; Greer et al, 2016). In most cases, investigators have reached the desired terminal schedule (e.g., S D components lasting 1 min and S Ī” components lasting 4 min) with rates of destructive behavior averaging 80 to 100% below baseline rates (e.g., Greer et al, 2016; Rooker et al, 2013).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%