2010
DOI: 10.3758/mc.38.5.591
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Structural properties of spatial representations in blind people: Scanning images constructed from haptic exploration or from locomotion in a 3-D audio virtual environment

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
60
0
7

Year Published

2011
2011
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
5
4
1

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 74 publications
(71 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
1
60
0
7
Order By: Relevance
“…However, they did not measure response latencies, so participants who ignored the rotation may have had sufficient time to mentally realign their orientation. People who are blind from birth do not show this discrepancy between imagined and actual movement; they have poor performance in both cases (Rieser et al, 1986) but may also form spatial representations through other means that can at times be superior to those of sighted individuals (Afonso et al, 2010). In contrast, late-blind people show the same discrepancy as sighted individuals (Rieser et al, 1986).…”
Section: Idiothetic Information In Spatial Updatingmentioning
confidence: 76%
“…However, they did not measure response latencies, so participants who ignored the rotation may have had sufficient time to mentally realign their orientation. People who are blind from birth do not show this discrepancy between imagined and actual movement; they have poor performance in both cases (Rieser et al, 1986) but may also form spatial representations through other means that can at times be superior to those of sighted individuals (Afonso et al, 2010). In contrast, late-blind people show the same discrepancy as sighted individuals (Rieser et al, 1986).…”
Section: Idiothetic Information In Spatial Updatingmentioning
confidence: 76%
“…The goal is thus to complement and go beyond what is possible with the use of traditional assistive devices (e.g., white cane or guide dog) and not to replace them. As an example, VI people use most of the time egocentric spatial representation strategies [16][17][18]. This involves that the various paths they know throughout their environment are well learned.…”
Section: Analysis: User Task Contextmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The results highlight the role of 3D audio cues to convey spatial information. The use of spatial audio in a navigation aid has recently been investigated by while (Afonso, Blum, Katz, Tarroux, Borst, & Denis, 2010) used tactile or spatialized audio renderings to evaluate spatial cognition skills in visually impaired and blindfolded individuals. Similarly, (Lahav & D. Mioduser, 2008) and (Traylor & Tan, 2002) highlighted the role of kinesthetic and vibrotactile cues supporting cognitive mapping of A c c e p t e d M a n u s c r i p t 6 unknown 3D spaces and conveying navigation and spatial orientation information through guidance strategies.…”
Section: Strategies For Exploration and Target Searchmentioning
confidence: 99%