1993
DOI: 10.1006/jmla.1993.1027
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Splitting the Differences: A Structural Alignment View of Similarity

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

23
298
1
3

Year Published

2002
2002
2013
2013

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 266 publications
(331 citation statements)
references
References 39 publications
23
298
1
3
Order By: Relevance
“…processing (e.g., Gentner et al, 2011;Kotovsky & Gentner, 1996;Markman & Gentner, 1993a), we would expect that the kinds of skeletal configurations that are highlighted as in previous comparisons would be more likely to be picked out in new cases. Such effects may be similar to chess experts' use of their knowledge of allowable spatial configurations in chess to quickly and accurately identify violations (Chase & Simon, 1973).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 87%
“…processing (e.g., Gentner et al, 2011;Kotovsky & Gentner, 1996;Markman & Gentner, 1993a), we would expect that the kinds of skeletal configurations that are highlighted as in previous comparisons would be more likely to be picked out in new cases. Such effects may be similar to chess experts' use of their knowledge of allowable spatial configurations in chess to quickly and accurately identify violations (Chase & Simon, 1973).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 87%
“…On a conceptual level, feature complementarity can be related to the notion of alignability, which has been advanced by research on structural alignment in similarity comparisons (Gentner 1983;Gentner and Markman 1994). It has been proposed that the structural alignment process yields two types of relationships: commonalities and differences, which, in turn, can be classified as alignable or nonalignable (Markman and Gentner 1993). Both alignable and nonalignable differences describe the relationship between the attribute levels of the compared objects; however, nonalignable differences describe a scenario in which a given feature is present in one of the objects and absent in the other, whereas alignable differences describe a scenario in which objects have different (but nonzero) levels of a given attribute.…”
Section: Feature Complementarity In Choicementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, noncomplementary features (e.g., different flavors) are considered alignable because all alternatives have corresponding features. Similarly, as long as all options have nonzero levels of performance on the attributes defined by the complementary features, these features would be considered alignable as well (Markman and Gentner 1993). To illustrate, option A's superiority on a given attribute (e.g., cavity prevention) does not imply that option B entirely lacks that attribute, but simply that option B underperforms on that attribute relative to option A.…”
Section: Feature Complementarity In Choicementioning
confidence: 99%
“…If words are GTs that represent relations efficiently, then regardless of the semantics of the relational labels, they should provide a benefit. Especially when working together with comparison (Doumas et al, 2008;Markman & Gentner, 1993) to drive the discovery of relational similarity, the presence of GTs that can represent these extracted relations may be beneficial. More alignable (relationally comparable) SDT stories will benefit from GTs more than less alignable SDT stories.…”
Section: Rationale Of Experimentsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Relational thinking plays a central role in human cognition. It underlies our ability to perceive and understand the spatial relations among an object's parts (Hummel, 2000;Hummel & Biederman, 1992;Hummel & Stankewicz, 1996), comprehend arrangements of objects in scenes (Green & Hummel, 2006;Markman & Gentner, 1993;Richland, Morrison, & Holyoak, 2006), and comprehend abstract analogies between otherwise very different situations or systems of knowledge (e.g., between the structure of the solar system and the structure of the atom; Gentner, 1983;Gick & Holyoak, 1980, 1983Holyoak & Thagard, 1995). However, despite its centrality in human cognition, relational thinking is cognitively demanding.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%