2011
DOI: 10.3758/s13414-011-0203-x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Seeing while acting: hand movements can modulate attentional capture by motion onset

Abstract: The onset of new motion has been shown to be a very robust cause of attentional capture, generating a processing advantage for the location of motion onset regardless of the observer's concurrent goal. The present study, motivated by the common-coding account of action and perception, examined whether the effect of motion onset on visual attention can be modulated by the observer's mode of action. Specifically, the commoncoding account predicts that preparing an action can render the features that are used in … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
9
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 49 publications
1
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…These effects are often interpreted as being due to a facilitative effect from action preparation on the perception of action compatible stimuli ( Bortoletto, Mattingley, & Cunnington, 2011 ; Craighero et al, 2002 , 1999 ). The results of these studies are largely in agreement with the PCM, because most of them show facilitative effects, and the compatible dimensions are mostly spatial (see Gozli & Pratt, 2011 , for an exception). We did, however, not focus the review so far on single tasks, because the interpretation of compatibility effects in these tasks as motorvisual effects has frequently been criticized (e.g., Miall et al, 2006 ; Vogt, Taylor, & Hopkins, 2003 ).…”
Section: Extending the Scope Of The Pcmsupporting
confidence: 68%
“…These effects are often interpreted as being due to a facilitative effect from action preparation on the perception of action compatible stimuli ( Bortoletto, Mattingley, & Cunnington, 2011 ; Craighero et al, 2002 , 1999 ). The results of these studies are largely in agreement with the PCM, because most of them show facilitative effects, and the compatible dimensions are mostly spatial (see Gozli & Pratt, 2011 , for an exception). We did, however, not focus the review so far on single tasks, because the interpretation of compatibility effects in these tasks as motorvisual effects has frequently been criticized (e.g., Miall et al, 2006 ; Vogt, Taylor, & Hopkins, 2003 ).…”
Section: Extending the Scope Of The Pcmsupporting
confidence: 68%
“…Features in an event file are thought to be shielded against all other concurrent cognitive processes, including perceptual processes. Thus, when selecting an action with a certain feature (e.g., “left”) in one task, perceptions of stimuli including congruent features (e.g., “left”) in another task are impaired (e.g., Eder & Klauer, 2007, 2009; Gozli & Pratt, 2011; James & Gauthier, 2009; Kunde & Wühr, 2004; Müsseler & Hommel, 1997b).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Axis-specific interference has been previously reported [ 43 , 44 ], and could be explained in at least three ways. First, the involvement of the vertical spatial codes in processing the concept could render those spatial codes less available for the concurrent perceptual task [ 45 , 46 ]. Second, the need to differentiate and keep separate the two subtasks, the conceptual judgment and the visual search, might have inhibited the features that they share in common [ 47 , 48 ].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This observation suggests that concepts either facilitated visual selection of items along the horizontal axis or interfered with visual selection of items along the vertical axis. Although both alternatives are logically possible, there is much stronger precedence for the latter possibility in the literature [ 26 , 39 , 40 , 43 , 44 , 45 ].…”
Section: Experiments 2: Cues Absentmentioning
confidence: 99%