2001
DOI: 10.1139/z01-017
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Repetitive calls of juvenile Richardson's ground squirrels (Spermophilus richardsonii) communicate response urgency

Abstract: Richardson's ground squirrels, Spermophilus richardsonii, produce both repetitive and non-repetitive antipredator calls. While many hypotheses have been advanced to explain non-repetitive calls, the function of repetitive calling has received relatively little attention. We presented juvenile Richardson's ground squirrels with a predator model at distances ranging from 1 to 8 m and recorded the subsequent repetitive calls on digital audiotape. The rate of calling was inversely correlated with the distance betw… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

2
72
2

Year Published

2003
2003
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 59 publications
(77 citation statements)
references
References 21 publications
2
72
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Consistent with this, many signalling systems have been demonstrated not to be reflexive, but rather, sensitive to intrinsic and extrinsic factors, such as the signaler's life history (Abrams 1983), the presence or absence of a suitable audience (Karakashian et al 1988), or the location of a given predator (Warkentin et al 2001). The great tit (Parus major, Linnaeus, 1758), for example, produces an 8--kHz 'seet' alarm call when its primary predator, the European sparrowhawk (Accipiter nisus, Linnaeus, 1758), is distant, but halts signal production when that predator approaches the signal's audible range (Klump and Shalter 1984).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 80%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…Consistent with this, many signalling systems have been demonstrated not to be reflexive, but rather, sensitive to intrinsic and extrinsic factors, such as the signaler's life history (Abrams 1983), the presence or absence of a suitable audience (Karakashian et al 1988), or the location of a given predator (Warkentin et al 2001). The great tit (Parus major, Linnaeus, 1758), for example, produces an 8--kHz 'seet' alarm call when its primary predator, the European sparrowhawk (Accipiter nisus, Linnaeus, 1758), is distant, but halts signal production when that predator approaches the signal's audible range (Klump and Shalter 1984).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 80%
“…By integrating information about predator distance (Warkentin et al 2001) and the presence or absence of nearby familiar or related conspecifics (Hare 1998b;Hare and Atkins 2001), callers could use exact information (but see Koops and Abrahams 1998) or simple rules of thumb (Bouskila and Blumstein 1992) to select the optimal strategy for a given situation (Maynard Smith 1978;Abrams 1983). A signaler's strategy could even be adaptable to situational changes, such as shifts in predator location or changes in the abundance of nearby squirrels, if the caller continually assesses the situation (e.g.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…While both call types warn conspecifics of potential danger (e.g. Sherman 1977;Warkentin et al 2001), the nature and extent of information encoded within repeated alarm calls remains unknown. The tonic communication hypothesis predicts that repetition of syllables serves to prolong residual vigilance in receivers during and after a predatory threat.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%