2020
DOI: 10.1038/s41467-020-20142-y
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Quantifying and addressing the prevalence and bias of study designs in the environmental and social sciences

Abstract: Building trust in science and evidence-based decision-making depends heavily on the credibility of studies and their findings. Researchers employ many different study designs that vary in their risk of bias to evaluate the true effect of interventions or impacts. Here, we empirically quantify, on a large scale, the prevalence of different study designs and the magnitude of bias in their estimates. Randomised designs and controlled observational designs with pre-intervention sampling were used by just 23% of in… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

2
71
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 72 publications
(86 citation statements)
references
References 71 publications
(52 reference statements)
2
71
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The other perception, that non-English-language studies tend to adopt less robust designs, seems to be supported by our results, although a reasonable number of non-English-language studies with robust designs also exist, especially in Portuguese (25 studies with Randomised Controlled Trial) and Spanish (13 studies with Before-After-Control-Impact and three with Randomised Controlled Trial). Scientific evidence presented in non-English-language studies could thus be lower in quality, and suffer from more serious biases, on average, compared to that provided by English-language studies [24]. This difference in evidence quality between English-language and non-English language studies is likely to create a trade-off in evidence-poor regions, between the availability of context-specific evidence and the quality of evidence; for some species and locations, the only available evidence might be found in non-English-language studies based on less robust designs [25].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The other perception, that non-English-language studies tend to adopt less robust designs, seems to be supported by our results, although a reasonable number of non-English-language studies with robust designs also exist, especially in Portuguese (25 studies with Randomised Controlled Trial) and Spanish (13 studies with Before-After-Control-Impact and three with Randomised Controlled Trial). Scientific evidence presented in non-English-language studies could thus be lower in quality, and suffer from more serious biases, on average, compared to that provided by English-language studies [24]. This difference in evidence quality between English-language and non-English language studies is likely to create a trade-off in evidence-poor regions, between the availability of context-specific evidence and the quality of evidence; for some species and locations, the only available evidence might be found in non-English-language studies based on less robust designs [25].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To test whether there was a difference in study designs adopted between studies in different languages, we only included studies based on one of the following five designs: After, Before-After (BA), Control-Impact (CI), Before-After-Control-Impact (BACI), and Randomised Controlled Trial (RCT). These study designs were recorded as an ordinal variable with RCT being the least biased design, followed by BACI, CI, BA, and After, based on results from [24]. Considering that English-language studies in English-speaking countries (especially the UK and the US) may adopt more robust study designs than English-language studies in other countries, English-language studies were further divided into two groups; studies conducted in countries where English is an official language (“English – official”), and studies in all other countries (“English – others”), using information on countries’ official languages in [40].…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The first knowledge gap is spatial: there is limited information from less seasonal regions (figure 1a), including critical areas under threat of forest degradation such as the Purus-Madeira interfluve, which links the central and southeastern Amazon [34]. The second gap is temporal: few studies show temporal trajectories of changes in forest functioning after fire [9,20,24] despite this being crucial to quantify impacts with less bias [35]. The third gap is in understanding to what extent plant morphological traits can avoid the effects of fire intensity on post-fire mortality [21][22][23] and post-fire recovery [24].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…number of clusters). One possible workaround could be to iterate the clustering step (similar to VoPo [ 36 ]) and differential abundance analysis, whose results can then be combined to obtain regions in marker space of high probability of an association with the corresponding covariate.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%