2010
DOI: 10.1167/10.13.20
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Perceptual learning of parametric face categories leads to the integration of high-level class-based information but not to high-level pop-out

Abstract: To date, the relative contribution of the different levels of the visual hierarchy during perceptual decisions remains unclear. Typical models of visual processing, with the reverse hierarchy theory (RHT) as a prominent example, strongly emphasize the role of higher levels and interpret lower levels as sequence of simple feature detectors. Here, we investigate this issue based on two analyses. Using a novel combination of perceptual learning based on two classes of parametric faces and a subsequent odd-one-out… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
2
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
3
1

Relationship

0
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(2 citation statements)
references
References 34 publications
0
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In an alternative view, aversive learning consists of recovering the vector of features that defines the adversity gradient (Figure 6B, black arrow ), leading to an increased saliency for discriminative features that separate best the harm- and safety-predicting prototypes. This feature vector could overlap with categorical knowledge that is either naturally present (such as gender, ethnicity or emotional expression), or learned with experience (Dunsmoor and Murphy, 2015; Kietzmann and König, 2010; Qu et al, 2016). While both scenarios lead to an increased separation between the CS+ and CS– poles, as observed in the present study, they have divergent predictions when tested with stimuli organized in three concentric circles (Figure 6B).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In an alternative view, aversive learning consists of recovering the vector of features that defines the adversity gradient (Figure 6B, black arrow ), leading to an increased saliency for discriminative features that separate best the harm- and safety-predicting prototypes. This feature vector could overlap with categorical knowledge that is either naturally present (such as gender, ethnicity or emotional expression), or learned with experience (Dunsmoor and Murphy, 2015; Kietzmann and König, 2010; Qu et al, 2016). While both scenarios lead to an increased separation between the CS+ and CS– poles, as observed in the present study, they have divergent predictions when tested with stimuli organized in three concentric circles (Figure 6B).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…gender and age). Across interleaved conditioning and test phases, participants can be given the possibility to extract the underlying category structure 53 . Importantly, it is crucial to pit the category membership of faces against perceptual similarities to investigate their independent contributions over the course of the learning.…”
Section: Two-stage Model Of Fear Generalizationmentioning
confidence: 99%