2007
DOI: 10.3758/bf03196810
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Neurophysiological evidence for transfer appropriate processing of memory: Processing versus feature similarity

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

5
31
3

Year Published

2011
2011
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 35 publications
(40 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
5
31
3
Order By: Relevance
“…This pattern of ERPs was in agreement with previous studies investigating the ERP responses in relation to object manipulability (Petit et al, 2006;Proverbio et al, 2011). The N300 may be sensitive to the recoverability of the object structure reflecting the access to both the conceptual representation system (Damasio et al, 1996;McPherson and Holcomb, 1999;Schendan and Kutas, 2007) and the motoric properties of the manipulable tools (Petit et al, 2006;Proverbio et al, 2011). Considering that the N300 may index the automatic activation of the motor schemata linked to the object's use (Petit et al, 2006;Proverbio et al, 2011) the larger negative deflection found for low affording tools may, therefore, reflect additional processing for stimuli for which it is more difficult to detect the correct motor Fig.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This pattern of ERPs was in agreement with previous studies investigating the ERP responses in relation to object manipulability (Petit et al, 2006;Proverbio et al, 2011). The N300 may be sensitive to the recoverability of the object structure reflecting the access to both the conceptual representation system (Damasio et al, 1996;McPherson and Holcomb, 1999;Schendan and Kutas, 2007) and the motoric properties of the manipulable tools (Petit et al, 2006;Proverbio et al, 2011). Considering that the N300 may index the automatic activation of the motor schemata linked to the object's use (Petit et al, 2006;Proverbio et al, 2011) the larger negative deflection found for low affording tools may, therefore, reflect additional processing for stimuli for which it is more difficult to detect the correct motor Fig.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…It is likely that high affording/high attractive tools evaluation might exert additive effects on the neural activity. Hence, we hypothesized that high affording/ high attractive tools might elicit an amplitude enhancement of the P200, indexing the access to the object representation system (Amsel et al, in press;Luck and Hillyard, 1994;Phillips and Takeda, 2009;Schendan and Lucia, 2010) and of the fronto-central N300, indexing conceptual processing (Schendan and Kutas, 2007), motor representations processing (Proverbio et al, 2011;Petit et al, 2006) and esthetic appreciation process (Jacobsen and Höfel, 2003). Moreover, the evaluation of high affording/high attractive tools may enhance the P300 component that represents an attentional updating process (Polich, 2007) and has been shown to be sensitive to positive arousing stimuli (DelPlanque et al, 2004(DelPlanque et al, , 2006De Tommaso et al, 2008).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Such early grouping effects have been localized to the calcarie cortex (Han, Jiang, Mao, Humphrey, & Qin, 2005). However, perceptual grouping operations have also been described in later ERP components (Casco, Campana, Han, & Guzzon, 2009;Han & Humphrey, 2007;Han, Jiang, Mao, Humphrey, & Qin, 2005;Schendan & Kutas, 2007b). For instance, Schedan and Kutas (2007) observed an occipitotemporoparietal P2 repetition effect when similar perceptual grouping processes of good continuation and closure were repeatedly engaged between the study and the test experiment.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…For instance, Schedan and Kutas (2007) observed an occipitotemporoparietal P2 repetition effect when similar perceptual grouping processes of good continuation and closure were repeatedly engaged between the study and the test experiment. Most likely, the perceptual processes occurring in the P200 time window are already under the influence of visual object knowledge that directs the attention to salient image features (Schendan & Kutas, 2007b) and reflects the feedback from high-level brain structures (Han & Humphrey, 2007;Han, Jiang, Mao, Humphrey, & Qin, 2005).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Object processing in the visual cortex involves a cascade of neural events, which are reflected in the P1-N1-P2 ERP complex (Murray et al, 2006;Rousselet et al, 2007;Smith et al, 2007;Barbeau et al, 2008;Okazaki et al, 2008). The P2 component, in particular, might reflect grouping processes (Schendan and Kutas, 2007;Schendan and Lucia, 2010) as well as high-level object processing (Halit et al, 2000;Itier and Taylor, 2002;Latinus and Taylor, 2005;Mercure et al, 2008) and might be modulated by the recurrent and feedback processes within the visual cortical hierarchy in the case of increased processing demands, such as during visual masking (Kotsoni et al, 2007). Therefore, at this point it is not clear to what extent the modulation of the P2 component in the study by Philiastides et al (2006) reflected the effect of noise on visual processing or on the processes related to detection of overall task difficulty and allocation of attentional resources.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%