2017
DOI: 10.1590/1982-43272766201712
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Mothers-Offspring Resemblance in Intelligence and Its Relationship to Socioeconomic Status

Abstract: Abstract:Kinship studies have shown that most cognitive ability variances are attributable to genotypic variance. Additionally, kinship correlations are generally increased when tasks that are highly g loaded are considered, a result known as the "Jensen effect". Alternatively, some studies have suggested socioeconomic status as an important factor for explaining differences in cognition. The present study investigated these premises in a sample of 141 mothers (mean age = 36.6; SD = 6.0) and their offspring (5… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2

Citation Types

0
2
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
3

Relationship

0
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 3 publications
(2 citation statements)
references
References 41 publications
0
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Other studies of the Scarr-Rowe effect have used different behavior-genetic techniques, including adoption designs ( Loehlin et al, 2022 ), parent-offspring resemblance measures ( Nagoshi and Johnson, 2005 ; Flores-Mendoza et al, 2017 ), and molecular methods such as single nucleotide polymorphism heritabilities and polygenic scoring (PGS) ( Woodley of Menie et al, 2018 ; Rask-Andersen et al, 2021 ; Woodley of Menie et al, 2021 ; Peñaherrera-Aguirre et al, 2022 ). The results of these studies have also been mixed, with some (e.g., Woodley of Menie et al, 2018 , 2021 ; Peñaherrera-Aguirre et al, 2022 ) finding evidence for the effect in both younger and older US cohorts, some finding evidence for the effect in (younger) Brazilian cohorts ( Flores-Mendoza et al, 2017 ), some finding no evidence for the effect in (older) US cohorts ( Nagoshi and Johnson, 2005 ; Loehlin et al, 2022 ), and another finding evidence for the opposite effect in a large middle-aged UK cohort ( Rask-Andersen et al, 2021 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Other studies of the Scarr-Rowe effect have used different behavior-genetic techniques, including adoption designs ( Loehlin et al, 2022 ), parent-offspring resemblance measures ( Nagoshi and Johnson, 2005 ; Flores-Mendoza et al, 2017 ), and molecular methods such as single nucleotide polymorphism heritabilities and polygenic scoring (PGS) ( Woodley of Menie et al, 2018 ; Rask-Andersen et al, 2021 ; Woodley of Menie et al, 2021 ; Peñaherrera-Aguirre et al, 2022 ). The results of these studies have also been mixed, with some (e.g., Woodley of Menie et al, 2018 , 2021 ; Peñaherrera-Aguirre et al, 2022 ) finding evidence for the effect in both younger and older US cohorts, some finding evidence for the effect in (younger) Brazilian cohorts ( Flores-Mendoza et al, 2017 ), some finding no evidence for the effect in (older) US cohorts ( Nagoshi and Johnson, 2005 ; Loehlin et al, 2022 ), and another finding evidence for the opposite effect in a large middle-aged UK cohort ( Rask-Andersen et al, 2021 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Parent-offspring resemblance cannot differentiate between genetic and environmental influences on cognitive ability ( Devlin et al, 1997 ). But, as already noted, parent-offspring resemblance has been used in previous studies of the Scarr-Rowe effect in the absence of twin or other types of familial data that would permit the direct estimation of behavior-genetic variance components (e.g., Nagoshi and Johnson, 2005 ; Flores-Mendoza et al, 2017 ). A finding of moderation involving a simple parent-offspring resemblance model must therefore be interpreted with caution, as moderation stemming from SES on additive heritability is only one pathway that can influence the degree of parent-offspring resemblance.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%