2023
DOI: 10.1002/jeab.893
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Measuring context–response associations that drive habits

Jennifer S. Labrecque,
Kristen M. Lee,
Wendy Wood

Abstract: People achieve important life outcomes of health, financial security, and productivity by repeating operant behavior. To identify whether such operants reflect goal pursuit or habit, the present research introduces a new paradigm that yields objective measures of learning and controls for the motivations of goal pursuit. In two experiments, participants practiced a sequential task of making sushi and then completed a test of the strength of cue–response (habit) associations in memory. Finally, they repeated th… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
1
0

Year Published

2024
2024
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(2 citation statements)
references
References 41 publications
0
1
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Some commentators have proposed a distinction between habits that trigger (i.e., 'habitually instigate') an episode of behaviour, and habits that automate performance (i.e., 'habitually execute' the action; Gardner et al, 2016). This distinction can be depicted sequentially; adopting an action phases approach (e.g., Heckhausen, 2007), 'instigation habits' effectively bridge the gap between pre-action and action, by selecting and generating a commitment to 'taking a shower', whereas 'execution habits' allow smooth movement through the action phase to completion (e.g., Labrecque et al, 2024). Alternatively, the instigation-execution distinction can be portrayed hierarchically, with 'instigation habits' initiating a higher-order action, within which is nested a series of lower-order actions facilitated by 'execution habits' (Figure 2).…”
Section: Multiple Habits Can Contribute To Any Given Actionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some commentators have proposed a distinction between habits that trigger (i.e., 'habitually instigate') an episode of behaviour, and habits that automate performance (i.e., 'habitually execute' the action; Gardner et al, 2016). This distinction can be depicted sequentially; adopting an action phases approach (e.g., Heckhausen, 2007), 'instigation habits' effectively bridge the gap between pre-action and action, by selecting and generating a commitment to 'taking a shower', whereas 'execution habits' allow smooth movement through the action phase to completion (e.g., Labrecque et al, 2024). Alternatively, the instigation-execution distinction can be portrayed hierarchically, with 'instigation habits' initiating a higher-order action, within which is nested a series of lower-order actions facilitated by 'execution habits' (Figure 2).…”
Section: Multiple Habits Can Contribute To Any Given Actionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The basis of such automatic actions is drawn from the overall conceptualization of the habit construct as a stimulus–response effect based upon the connection between cues, contexts, and actions in associative memory [ 14 , 19 , 20 ]. Thus, a key thread in contemporary habit theory is that the development and enactment of more automatically performed actions should be contingent on the presence of consistent environmental or internal contexts in which a behavior is repeatedly performed [ 21 , 22 , 23 ], that is, a habit is formed when a behavior is repeated in the presence of a stable internal (e.g., mood) or external cue (e.g., place, time, and people), and the behavioral response becomes linked to the cue in associative memory. Once the cue and behavioral response become associated in memory, encountering that cue or context again should activate the learned stimulus–response effect and, thus, be sufficient to trigger the action chains encouraging behavioral enactment with little or no conscious deliberation [ 19 ].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%