1999
DOI: 10.1007/s003000050366
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Lateral line mediated rheotaxis in the Antarctic fish Pagothenia borchgrevinki

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

2
64
1

Year Published

2000
2000
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 53 publications
(67 citation statements)
references
References 14 publications
2
64
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Reef sounds show promise for reef orientation (14,16,42,43), but we still need to know how far the acoustic reef signal extends above the noise or how it relates to the hearing frequency range of these fishes (44); it may be limited to relatively short distances (Ϸ1 km) (25). Superficial lateral line is useful for rheotactic responses (45,46), particularly in shear layers near solid surfaces and among water masses. However, although directional per se, none of these acoustic and hydrodynamic senses would allow animals suspended in the water column without external frame of reference to select favorable currents.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Reef sounds show promise for reef orientation (14,16,42,43), but we still need to know how far the acoustic reef signal extends above the noise or how it relates to the hearing frequency range of these fishes (44); it may be limited to relatively short distances (Ϸ1 km) (25). Superficial lateral line is useful for rheotactic responses (45,46), particularly in shear layers near solid surfaces and among water masses. However, although directional per se, none of these acoustic and hydrodynamic senses would allow animals suspended in the water column without external frame of reference to select favorable currents.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…surrounding river banks), unless they are a benthic fish and in constant contact with the river bed. Thus, when deprived of visual cues, midwater fish may need to alter their behavior so that they are periodically in contact with the substrate to provide a (tactile) external frame of reference, as suggested previously (Lyon, 1904;Baker and Montgomery, 1999a). The effect of these fundamental differences in sensory input on behavioral output remains largely undocumented, except in the context of obstacle entrainment behaviors (Sutterlin and Waddy, 1975;Liao et al, 2003;Montgomery et al, 2003;Przybilla et al, 2010), which have a rheotactic component.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Optic flow cues appear to be sufficient for many species (Lyon, 1904), but not necessary, as rheotaxis persists in the absence of visual cues in species like the blind cavefish (Montgomery et al, 1997;Baker and Montgomery, 1999b). Nonvisual cues implicated in rheotaxis include tactile (Lyon, 1904;Baker and Montgomery, 1999a;Baker and Montgomery, 1999b), vestibular (Pavlov and Tjurjukov, 1993) and lateral line (Montgomery et al, 1997;Baker and Montgomery, 1999a;Baker and Montgomery, 1999b) cues. In the absence of lateral line cues, substantial reduction in rheotactic performance (to chance levels) has been observed in blind cavefish and several other species, but only at flow speeds less than ~5cms -1 [~1fish body length (BL)s -1 ] (Montgomery et al, 1997).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In the near field, vision and the lateral line system are of primary importance (Giske et al, 1998). The mechanosensory lateral line is used to monitor the local hydrodynamic environment, and is sensitive to water velocity and thus important for rheotaxis (Montgomery et al, 1997;Baker and Montgomery, 1999) and the acceleration component of oscillatory water flow (Montgomery et al, 1997;. Studies designed to identify the relative roles of vision and the lateral line system have often relied on experimental manipulation of the fish, e.g.…”
Section: What Is the Optimal Design For Fish Passage Structures?mentioning
confidence: 99%