2019
DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arz079
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

It’s a trap! Invasive common mynas learn socially about control-related cues

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 75 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Mynas were also good observational learners. They remembered a "dangerous" human after observing a staged catching event (Diquelou and Griffin, 2019) and became more wary (increase in locomotion) in the location of such an aversive event (Griffin and Boyce, 2009). Observing the behavior of an alarmed conspecific when the source of the threat was not visible, however, did not affect the observers behavior (Griffin et al, 2010).…”
Section: Vertebratesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Mynas were also good observational learners. They remembered a "dangerous" human after observing a staged catching event (Diquelou and Griffin, 2019) and became more wary (increase in locomotion) in the location of such an aversive event (Griffin and Boyce, 2009). Observing the behavior of an alarmed conspecific when the source of the threat was not visible, however, did not affect the observers behavior (Griffin et al, 2010).…”
Section: Vertebratesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While a number of studies have demonstrated that animals learn socially about novel predators (Griffin, 2004), only a handful of studies to date have examined the importance of social learning in allowing animals to avoid threats encountered in urban environments. For example, common mynas (A. tristis) appear to use social cues to infer danger associated with pest control measures, showing higher wariness of a location after seeing a conspecific being captured in that location (Griffin and Boyce, 2009;Diquelou and Griffin, 2019). Studies of urban corvids also provide evidence of social learning: American crows (C. brachyrhynchos) will gather to mob individual people seen capturing crows; this behaviour persists in future encounters with the person, and appears to spread to other birds not present at the initial capture event (Cornell et al, 2012).…”
Section: Social Learningmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Changes in space use can also alter the ecological impact of a species. For example, common mynas undergo heavy trapping pressure in suburban habitats where the human demographic is particularly intolerant of the species (Diquelou, 2017). In the long-term, this could cause mynas to invade more pristine habitats to escape trapping pressure, which would increase the likelihood of competitive interactions with native secondary cavity-nesting birds.…”
Section: Potential Consequences Of Behavioral Responsesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Attention to whether and how wildlife is changing as a consequence of lethal control is quasi inexistent, even though some have begun examining how removing invasive and pest species might alter ecological function, and warning that such interventions might have unexpected/undesirable consequences (Zavaleta et al, 2001;Wallach et al, 2010;Kopf et al, 2017). The possibility that wildlife responds adaptively, which is relevant to both maintaining the effectiveness of population management, and understanding more generally how wild populations respond to anthropic influences, has been almost completely neglected until very recently (Côté et al, 2014;Diquelou et al, 2018;Diquelou and Griffin, 2019). Furthermore, a broad understanding of the proximate mechanisms underpinning change and how this knowledge can be used by managers has so far been overlooked (Diquelou et al, 2018;Závorka et al, 2018).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%