1995
DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.21.4.914
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Influence of case type, word frequency, and exposure duration on visual word recognition.

Abstract: The authors report 4 lexical decision experiments in which case type, word frequency, and exposure duration were varied. These data indicated that there is a larger mixed-case disadvantage for nonwords than for words for longer duration presentations of targets. However, when targets were presented for 100 ms (followed by a postdisplay pattern mask), a larger mixed-case disadvantage occurred for words than for nonwords. For word frequency, the data from Experiments 1, 2, and 3 revealed a slightly larger mixed-… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

6
83
1

Year Published

1998
1998
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 64 publications
(90 citation statements)
references
References 45 publications
6
83
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Namely, the data can be explained by the two-stage model proposed by Allen, Wallace and Weber (1995) and Yap and Balota (2007). The first stage involves stimulus normalization, when familiarity-based information is assessed (e.g., the orthographic similarity of a letter string to a word), and the second stage involves stimulus categorization/lexical access.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Namely, the data can be explained by the two-stage model proposed by Allen, Wallace and Weber (1995) and Yap and Balota (2007). The first stage involves stimulus normalization, when familiarity-based information is assessed (e.g., the orthographic similarity of a letter string to a word), and the second stage involves stimulus categorization/lexical access.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Words tend to have more familiar orthographic, phonologic and semantic patterns than nonwords. When nonwords are orthographically illegal, containing uncommon bigrams (e.g., CPTOA), and/or word stimuli are high frequency (e.g., TABLE), discriminations can be made on the basis of orthographic information alone (James, 1975;Allen et al, 1995). By contrast, when orthographically legal nonwords (e.g., PLASS) are included, or word stimuli are low frequency (e.g., KITE), orthographic analysis alone cannot discriminate reliably words from nonwords.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Following the original IA framework, more recent models of word recognition based on cascaded processing have incorporated the word-letter interactivity assumption (e.g., Coltheart, Curtis, Atkins, & Haller, 1993;Grainger & Jacobs, 1996). However, the above account of the word superiority effect and other related findings has been questioned on both empirical and simulation grounds (Allen, Wallace, & Weber, 1995;Jacobs & Grainger, 1992;Mewhort & Johns, 1988;Paap, Newsome, McDonald, & Schvaneveldt, 1982). Hence, it remains unclear whether reading really involves interactive processes allowing activation at a higher processing level to reverberate and influence activation at a lower processing level.…”
mentioning
confidence: 98%