2009
DOI: 10.1007/s10578-009-0123-3
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Infant Temperament in Russia, United States of America, and Israel: Differences and Similarities Between Russian-speaking Families

Abstract: The present study addresses cross-cultural differences between infants born to families of Russian immigrants in USA and Israel, as well as Russian families residing in Russia, with the emphasis on evaluating the impact of immigration and acculturation. Community samples of primary caregivers of infants between 3 and 12 months of age were recruited and asked to complete temperament (IBQ-R) and acculturation (SAM) questionnaires. Results support our hypotheses that cultural influences contribute to shaping infa… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

1
16
0
2

Year Published

2014
2014
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
3
2
2

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(19 citation statements)
references
References 25 publications
1
16
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Children’s social settings, familial customs, and caregivers’ psychosocial characteristics and beliefs may influence the meaning of children’s temperament (Cozzi et al, 2013; Gartstein, Peleg, Young, & Slobodskaya, 2009; Super & Harkness, 1986). Consequently, parents may engage in caregiving practices that reinforce the characteristics consistent with their values and those of their cultural group (Dragan et al, 2011; Gartstein et al, 2006, 2009), leading to cross-cultural differences in the development of temperament (Gartstein et al, 2006).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…Children’s social settings, familial customs, and caregivers’ psychosocial characteristics and beliefs may influence the meaning of children’s temperament (Cozzi et al, 2013; Gartstein, Peleg, Young, & Slobodskaya, 2009; Super & Harkness, 1986). Consequently, parents may engage in caregiving practices that reinforce the characteristics consistent with their values and those of their cultural group (Dragan et al, 2011; Gartstein et al, 2006, 2009), leading to cross-cultural differences in the development of temperament (Gartstein et al, 2006).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Children’s social settings, familial customs, and caregivers’ psychosocial characteristics and beliefs may influence the meaning of children’s temperament (Cozzi et al, 2013; Gartstein, Peleg, Young, & Slobodskaya, 2009; Super & Harkness, 1986). Consequently, parents may engage in caregiving practices that reinforce the characteristics consistent with their values and those of their cultural group (Dragan et al, 2011; Gartstein et al, 2006, 2009), leading to cross-cultural differences in the development of temperament (Gartstein et al, 2006). Support for this hypothesis comes from a number of studies among infants from different cultural groups that show differences on mean ratings across temperament scales (e.g., Cozzi et al, 2013; Dragan et al, 2011; Gartstein et al, 2006, 2009; Montirosso et al, 2011).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Gartstein, Peleg, Young, & Slobodskaya, 2009;Montirosso, Cozzi, Putnam, Gartstein, & Borgatti, 2010), longitudinal research (Putnam, Rothbart, & Gartstein, 2008), and in investigations of convergent validity in relation to observational procedures (Parade & Leerkes, 2008); moreover, the dimensions addressed through the questionnaire are recognized as representative of the temperamental dimensions in infancy (Zentner & Shiner, 2012).…”
Section: Adoptees' Temperamentmentioning
confidence: 99%