2016
DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1605115113
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

In-group defense, out-group aggression, and coordination failures in intergroup conflict

Abstract: Intergroup conflict persists when and because individuals make costly contributions to their group's fighting capacity, but how groups organize contributions into effective collective action remains poorly understood. Here we distinguish between contributions aimed at subordinating out-groups (out-group aggression) from those aimed at defending the in-group against possible out-group aggression (in-group defense). We conducted two experiments in which three-person aggressor groups confronted three-person defen… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
144
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1
1

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 120 publications
(155 citation statements)
references
References 62 publications
(80 reference statements)
6
144
0
Order By: Relevance
“…T he propensity to trust is essential for individuals to cooperate and for societies to prosper (1,2). Nevertheless, individuals also need to be equipped with decoding machinery in the brain, which allows them to quickly detect signals of danger (3), refrain from cooperation, and withhold trust (4). Among the many implicit cues that may inform assessments of someone's trustworthiness, the human eye region stands out as particularly salient and powerful.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…T he propensity to trust is essential for individuals to cooperate and for societies to prosper (1,2). Nevertheless, individuals also need to be equipped with decoding machinery in the brain, which allows them to quickly detect signals of danger (3), refrain from cooperation, and withhold trust (4). Among the many implicit cues that may inform assessments of someone's trustworthiness, the human eye region stands out as particularly salient and powerful.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Second, and possibly because of Behavioral and Brain Sciences these behavioral asymmetries, attackers are disproportionately less successful than defenders. Across a range of settings, from laboratory experiments to private sector competition to interstate warfare, we observed an attacker success rate averaging around 30% (De Dreu et al 2016a). Thus, the prevention of exploitation is more likely than subordination.…”
Section: Summary and Implicationsmentioning
confidence: 87%
“…As in individual attack-defense conflicts, intergroup conflict often arises because attackers seek an improvement over their status quo that defenders seek to protect. Thus, such conflicts are captured as team-level variants of the Best-Shot/Weakest Link Game (Chowdhury et al 2013;Chowdhury & Topolyan 2016a;Note 4) and the Intergroup Aggressor-Defender Contest (IAD-C; De Dreu et al 2016a) in which individual contributions are modeled continuously rather than binary. For example, assume an equal number of members in the two rivaling groups, with N = N A = N D .…”
Section: Games Of Attack and Defense Between Groupsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Here, subjects simultaneously wall sit and the winner, the person who endures longer in the position, takes the loser's money. In contrast to prior experiments on conflict, which typically use self-reported experience of fighting [62][63][64] or fighting as a potential loss of money [13,16,[65][66][67], our competitive wall sit captures key elements of real fights: it can be painful and requires both physical resilience and toughness.…”
Section: Measuring Toughness and Settling Fightsmentioning
confidence: 99%