1994
DOI: 10.1002/hbm.460020306
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Identification of early visual evoked potential generators by retinotopic and topographic analyses

Abstract: 4AbstrackThis study investigated the cortical sources of the early (5&250 ms) components of the pattern-onset visual evoked potential (VEP). VEPs were recorded in response to a small circular checkerboard stimulus that was flashed over a range of visual field positions. Temporally and spatially overlapping VEP components were distinguished by differences in retinotopic sensitivity and scalp topography, and by inverse dipole modeling. The C1 component (5Cr80 ms) was found to change its polarity and topography s… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

75
428
6
2

Year Published

1998
1998
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 499 publications
(513 citation statements)
references
References 36 publications
75
428
6
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Rather we propose that these components reflect object selection processes. 3 The timing of this function fits well with known visual processing times: (a) the earliest (125-175 ms) effects of perceptual categorization occur ∼100 ms after striate cortex activation (50-80 ms) [10], and (b) this visual categorization (i.e. discriminating faces from other objects) is easier and less specific than the basic level naming that overtly reveals identification success in the present study.…”
Section: Identification Componentry and Functional Significancesupporting
confidence: 83%
“…Rather we propose that these components reflect object selection processes. 3 The timing of this function fits well with known visual processing times: (a) the earliest (125-175 ms) effects of perceptual categorization occur ∼100 ms after striate cortex activation (50-80 ms) [10], and (b) this visual categorization (i.e. discriminating faces from other objects) is easier and less specific than the basic level naming that overtly reveals identification success in the present study.…”
Section: Identification Componentry and Functional Significancesupporting
confidence: 83%
“…First, we see the C1 component which flips polarity based in whether the eliciting stimulus appears in the upper or lower visual field and is believed to be generated by activity in primary visual cortex (Clark, Fan, & Hillyard, 1995; Clark & Hillyard, 1996; Estevez & Spekreijse, 1974; Jeffreys & Axford, 1972). This initial deflection is followed by the P1 and N1 components as information propagates through the visual system and perceptual analysis is performed (Heinze et al, 1994; Heinze, Mangun, & Hillyard, 1990; Luck, 1995; Vogel & Luck, 2000).…”
Section: Why Erps Are Well Suited To Study Perception and Attention?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The interest in the retinotopic variations of the visual ERPs has been almost exclusively focused on the C1 component (Ales et al, 2010b(Ales et al, , 2013Clark et al, 1995;Kelly et al, 2013aKelly et al, , 2013b. However, we still lack a detailed retinotopic map of later components, which are more commonly studied in the ERP literature, such as P1 and N1 (Di Russo et al, 2012;Klimesch, 2011;Mangun et al, 1998).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The second line of research has employed stimulation and recording protocols typically used in ERP studies (e.g. SOAs > 200 ms and multichannel scalp recordings; Clark et al, 1995;Di Russo et al, 2002. However, these studies have not exhaustively covered the visual field.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation