2014
DOI: 10.1002/evan.21403
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

How humans evolved large brains: Comparative evidence

Abstract: The human brain is about three times as large as that of our closest living relatives, the great apes. Overall brain size is a good predictor of cognitive performance in a variety of tests in primates.[1, 2] Therefore, hypotheses explaining the evolution of this remarkable difference have attracted much interest. In this review, we give an overview of the current evidence from comparative studies testing these hypotheses. If cognitive benefits are diverse and ubiquitous, it is possible that most of the variati… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

4
54
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
1
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 106 publications
(60 citation statements)
references
References 102 publications
4
54
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Whether such "intelligence" is thought to be composed of discrete but tightly coevolving innate modules (8) or a general-purpose capacity only "secondarily" specialized by experience (14), the premise is that bigger brains are generally advantageous but can only evolve under a narrow set of conditions. Lifespans must be long enough (i.e., mortality low enough) to reward early investments in growth and additional energetic costs must be accommodated through increased intake or reallocation (4). For many species, constraints such as small body size, unstable environments, and unavoidable mortality from predation and disease may simply make large brains impractical.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Whether such "intelligence" is thought to be composed of discrete but tightly coevolving innate modules (8) or a general-purpose capacity only "secondarily" specialized by experience (14), the premise is that bigger brains are generally advantageous but can only evolve under a narrow set of conditions. Lifespans must be long enough (i.e., mortality low enough) to reward early investments in growth and additional energetic costs must be accommodated through increased intake or reallocation (4). For many species, constraints such as small body size, unstable environments, and unavoidable mortality from predation and disease may simply make large brains impractical.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…An alternative is to consider cumulative culture, not as a unitary capacity that is either present or absent, but as a complex trait with a correspondingly complex history of gradual or piecemeal emergence. Although any event that altered the evolutionary cost/benefit analysis of either brain expansion generally (4,8) or high-fidelity social learning specifically (2, 16) could theoretically have initiated runaway biocultural coevolution in our lineage, there is no reason to assume this feedback would be indefinitely self-sustaining once initiated, nor that that it would necessarily produce constant increase as opposed to more complex dynamics. In fact, both comparative biological evidence (4) and cultural evolutionary models (19) indicate the potential for just such interactions and dynamics and this is entirely consistent with the emerging paleoanthropological picture of multilineal, intermittent, asynchronous change over human evolution (20,21).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Paternal care was likely a requirement for lengthened gestation and especially infancy -critical factors, which made human encephalization physically possible. For instance, based on the expensive brain hypothesis, advocated by K. Isler and C. Schaik [54][55][56], costs of larger brain in early hominids must be met by increased energy provision to female and offspring, possibly by paternal care. Likewise Kaplan [57,58] studying evolutionary roots of human social organization pointed out the essential role of males in energetics of human reproduction, in contrast to most higher primates.…”
Section: The Third Transition: Human Brain and Languagementioning
confidence: 99%
“…The energetic perspective is of great importance for understanding cognition, but it can also be used to understand emotional complexity (Isler & van Schaik, 2014). Through mediation by the medial prefrontal cortex, the intense sexual excitement radiates into the brain framework as a whole and thus spurs the flow of information about the state of affairs.…”
Section: The Brain As the Original Sexual Organmentioning
confidence: 99%