2013
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.02.005
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

From language-specific to shared syntactic representations: The influence of second language proficiency on syntactic sharing in bilinguals

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

33
196
5

Year Published

2014
2014
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
3
2
2

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 138 publications
(234 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
33
196
5
Order By: Relevance
“…Another way of testing for proficiency effects is by considering individual differences in the strength of priming. Thus, Bernolet et al (2013) observed stronger priming with genitives the more proficient the subjects were. Additionally, Hartsuiker and Bernolet (in press) reanalyzed the data of Schoonbaert et al (2007) and observed a similar modulation of priming by proficiency in cross-linguistic priming with dative sentences.…”
Section: Effects Of Proficiency and Linguistic Relatednessmentioning
confidence: 88%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Another way of testing for proficiency effects is by considering individual differences in the strength of priming. Thus, Bernolet et al (2013) observed stronger priming with genitives the more proficient the subjects were. Additionally, Hartsuiker and Bernolet (in press) reanalyzed the data of Schoonbaert et al (2007) and observed a similar modulation of priming by proficiency in cross-linguistic priming with dative sentences.…”
Section: Effects Of Proficiency and Linguistic Relatednessmentioning
confidence: 88%
“…Consistent with that account, priming was obtained between Dutch and German, which have the same word order. Bernolet et al (2013) observed that priming of genitive structures (the nun's hat vs. the hat of the nun) was multilingual cross-linguistic priming 12 much stronger within-L2 than between L1 and L2. They argued that the English genitive, which differs from its Dutch counterpart in morphology and usage constraints, is learned rather late and so is not yet shared in the participants with relatively low proficiency.…”
mentioning
confidence: 96%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Note that the effect of proficiency does not demonstrate any effect of proficiency on priming, and so we cannot conclude that proficiency affects the degree to which representations are integrated across languages (cf. Bernolet et al 2013). Of course, our participants were quite homogenous in terms of age, learning environment, and selfrated proficiency, and further work is needed to investigate this issue.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The age of onset of the L2 varies within studies, as does the exposure to the L2 (e.g., 6-20 years in McDonough, 2006), making it difficult to investigate the question of learning as a function of priming input. In one study designed to examine priming effects as a function of L2 proficiency, participants' self-reports revealed that the mean L2 proficiency in all groups tested was well above beginning levels (Bernolet, Hartsuiker, & Pickering, 2013), which also makes it difficult to engage in a discussion about the process of acquiring new forms.…”
Section: The Use Of the Syntactic Priming Paradigm With L2 Learnersmentioning
confidence: 98%