1999
DOI: 10.1006/brln.1999.2095
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Finnish Compound Structure: Experiments with a Morphologically Impaired Patient

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
5
0

Year Published

2003
2003
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 10 publications
1
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…If this were not the case, one would be unable to account for the differential effects obtained across compound constituents. In keeping with previous studies (Badecker, 2001;Blanken, 2000;Delazer & Semenza, 1998;Hittmair-Delazer et al, 1994;Mä kisalo et al, 1999), this systematic intra-compound variation thus clearly demonstrates that constituent activation is involved during the production of compounds in aphasia. The surfacing of compositional effects in this and earlier neuropsychological studies further confirms the structured nature of compound representation and processing advocated in the psycholinguistic literature (e.g., Libben & Jarema, 2006), indicating that similar principles govern compound production in language-impaired and intact populations.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 89%
“…If this were not the case, one would be unable to account for the differential effects obtained across compound constituents. In keeping with previous studies (Badecker, 2001;Blanken, 2000;Delazer & Semenza, 1998;Hittmair-Delazer et al, 1994;Mä kisalo et al, 1999), this systematic intra-compound variation thus clearly demonstrates that constituent activation is involved during the production of compounds in aphasia. The surfacing of compositional effects in this and earlier neuropsychological studies further confirms the structured nature of compound representation and processing advocated in the psycholinguistic literature (e.g., Libben & Jarema, 2006), indicating that similar principles govern compound production in language-impaired and intact populations.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 89%
“…HH's ability to read words was extensively studied over a long period of time, from 4 to over 10 years post onset (Laine, Niemi, Koivuselkä-Sallinen, & Hyönä, 1995;Laine & Niemi, 1997;Mäkisalo, Niemi, & Laine, 1999;Niemi, Laine, & Tuominen,1994). Clinically, his condition remained stable, and six-year followup data reported in Laine and Niemi (1997) indicated an essentially unchanged, severe deficit in reading.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, in a single-word reading task reported by Laine et al (1995), HH read aloud correctly 214/290 (74%) uninflected nouns while with inflected nouns, his success rate dropped to 40/290 correct (14%). In addition, compounds caused problems for HH: Mäkisalo et al (1999) found that in the reading of two-member Finnish noun+noun compounds (like eläin+tarha lit. 'animal+garden' , i.e., 'zoo') HH could read correctly somewhat less than half of the items.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A substantial body of evidence from the processing of compounds comes from studies investigating a single language . There have been nine languages tested so far: Dutch (Sandra 1990;Zwitserlood 1994), German (Bronk et al 2013), English (Libben 1998;Fiorentino & Fund-Reznicek 2009), Greek (Dalalakis 1999), Finnish (Mäkisalo et al 1999), Italian (Delazer & Semenza 1998;Mondini et al 2004;Marelli & Luzzatti 2012), Japanese (Kudo 1992), Chinese (Zhou & Marslen-Wilson 1994) and Hebrew (Berman & Clark 1989). Slavic single-language studies are seriously under-represented (Libben 2013).…”
Section: Factors Influencing the Processing Of Compoundsmentioning
confidence: 99%