2017
DOI: 10.1080/17470218.2016.1143956
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Distorted Body Representations in Healthy Cognition

Abstract: Delusions and misperceptions about the body are a conspicuous feature of numerous neurological and psychiatric conditions. In stark contrast to such pathological cases, the immediacy and familiarity of our ordinary experience of our body can make it seem as if our representation of our body is highly accurate, even infallible. Recent research has begun to demonstrate, however, that large and systematic distortions of body representation are a normal part of healthy cognition. Here, I will describe this researc… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
65
1

Year Published

2017
2017
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

3
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 60 publications
(72 citation statements)
references
References 81 publications
3
65
1
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, the overestimation of hand width relative to length mirrors findings of greater tactile spatial acuity in the medio-lateral than in the proximo-distal axis of the limbs (e.g., Weber, 1834/1996; Cody et al, 2008) and the fact that receptive fields of neurons in the spinal cord and cortex representing the limbs tend to be oval-shaped, with the long axis running along the proximo-distal limb axis (e.g., Powell and Mountcastle, 1959; Brooks et al, 1961; Brown et al, 1975; Alloway et al, 1989). Distortions in both tactile distance perception and position sense, however, are much smaller than would be predicted on the basis of receptive field size alone (Taylor-Clarke et al, 2004; Longo, 2017), suggesting that low-level distortions are at least partly corrected before affecting tactile distance perception and position sense. Thus, one possibility is that body representations underlying tactile distance perception and position sense are completely distinct, but both are shaped by lower-level somatosensory maps, and inherit their distortions.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 91%
“…For example, the overestimation of hand width relative to length mirrors findings of greater tactile spatial acuity in the medio-lateral than in the proximo-distal axis of the limbs (e.g., Weber, 1834/1996; Cody et al, 2008) and the fact that receptive fields of neurons in the spinal cord and cortex representing the limbs tend to be oval-shaped, with the long axis running along the proximo-distal limb axis (e.g., Powell and Mountcastle, 1959; Brooks et al, 1961; Brown et al, 1975; Alloway et al, 1989). Distortions in both tactile distance perception and position sense, however, are much smaller than would be predicted on the basis of receptive field size alone (Taylor-Clarke et al, 2004; Longo, 2017), suggesting that low-level distortions are at least partly corrected before affecting tactile distance perception and position sense. Thus, one possibility is that body representations underlying tactile distance perception and position sense are completely distinct, but both are shaped by lower-level somatosensory maps, and inherit their distortions.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 91%
“…It has been assumed that healthy people have accurate body representations and only very recently has research begun to challenge this assumption [5,11,32,33,35,5456]. While these studies have started to explore body size perception, only a few studies have looked specifically at the face or head.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Touch, however, can clearly also be used to perceive the extrinsic physical properties of objects. Longo, Azañón, and Haggard (2010) argued that perceiving the metric properties of objects touching the skin requires that immediate sensory signals be combined with (Berryman, Yau, & Hsiao, 2006), sounds produced by action (Tajadura-Jiménez et al, 2012, 2015, the rubber hand illusion (Bruno & Bertamini, 2010), and tool use (Canzoneri et al, 2013;Miller, Longo, & Saygin, 2014, 2017. Thus, in analogy with the modulation of perceived passability of apertures when apparent eyeheight was altered shown by Warren and Whang (1987), these results show that experimental manipulations of represented body size alter perceived tactile distance.…”
Section: Statement Of Public Significancementioning
confidence: 91%
“…In owl monkeys, for example, the magnification levels on different skin surfaces differ by two orders of magnitude (e.g., Sur, Merzenich, & Kaas, 1980). While the differences in perceived tactile distance across skin surfaces seen in Weber's illusion parallel these differences, they are dramatically smaller in magnitude (Taylor-Clarke et al, 2004;Longo, 2017).…”
Section: Body-scaled Information For Tactile Perceptionmentioning
confidence: 99%