1990
DOI: 10.1037/0033-295x.97.2.253
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--A model and taxonomy.

Abstract: The classic problem of stimulus-response (S-R) compatibility (SRC) is addressed. A cognitive model is proposed that views the stimulus and response sets in S-R ensembles as categories with dimensions that may or may not overlap. If they do overlap, the task may be compatible or incompatible, depending on the assigned S-R mapping. If they do not overlap, the task is noncompatible regardless of the assigned mapping. The overlapping dimensions may be relevant or not. The model provides a systematic account of SRC… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

96
1,811
12
10

Year Published

1996
1996
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 1,850 publications
(1,957 citation statements)
references
References 134 publications
96
1,811
12
10
Order By: Relevance
“…Our valence manipulation appears to tap these processing aspects, because in the bivalent condition, stimulus selection, stimulus-response transformation, and response selection have to proceed in the face of a distractor (such as in the Stroop color-word task, see MacLeod, 1991, for review). Similar processes may underlie stimulus-response compatibility effects (for review, see Kornblum et al, 1990;Lien and Proctor, 2002). However, our data clearly indicate that this aspect is the same across testing conditions (i.e., no interaction including the valence variable and the experimental group variable was significant), so it cannot account for the slowing.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 54%
“…Our valence manipulation appears to tap these processing aspects, because in the bivalent condition, stimulus selection, stimulus-response transformation, and response selection have to proceed in the face of a distractor (such as in the Stroop color-word task, see MacLeod, 1991, for review). Similar processes may underlie stimulus-response compatibility effects (for review, see Kornblum et al, 1990;Lien and Proctor, 2002). However, our data clearly indicate that this aspect is the same across testing conditions (i.e., no interaction including the valence variable and the experimental group variable was significant), so it cannot account for the slowing.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 54%
“…The distractors represented either the truth or lie response to these questions (or were neutral symbols). Even though the distractors were task-irrelevant, we expected that they would bias responding because they overlapped with the task-relevant responses (Kornblum, Hasbroucq, & Osman, 1990). Based on the assumption that lie construction may involve a two-step process, we expected to observe an effect of covert congruency in reaction times and errors.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, stimulus-response compatibility effects (e.g., Fitts & Seeger, 1953;Kornblum, et al, 1990) occur when the features of the stimulus matches those of the response, producing faster RTs and higher accuracy scores though these effects can be explained if it is assumed that some responses are more readily retrieved than others given a particular stimulus cue (Wuhr & Ansorge, 2007). However, interactions between task-irrelevant stimulus features and the responses features on RT (Simon, 1969;Simon & Rudell, 1967) are less conveniently accommodated as are interactions between the features of stimuli of one task and the features of responses of a temporally overlapping second task (Hommel, 1998).…”
Section: Discrete Models Of Response Selectionmentioning
confidence: 99%