2009
DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.10.019
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Differential effects of methamphetamine and cocaine on conditioned place preference and locomotor activity in adult and adolescent male rats

Abstract: SummaryHuman and animal laboratory studies show that adolescents and adults respond differently to drugs and that drug administration during adolescence leads to different behavioral effects than during adulthood. Although there are a number of studies on the effects of cocaine, little is known about the effects of methamphetamine in adolescent vs adult rats. In the present study, sensitivity to the conditioned reward of multiple doses of methamphetamine or cocaine was evaluated in male adolescent (PND 34) and… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

13
92
3

Year Published

2010
2010
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 127 publications
(108 citation statements)
references
References 36 publications
13
92
3
Order By: Relevance
“…The doseeffect curves for a-PBP and a-PVP were both inverted U-shaped functions, with the low and high doses not producing conditioned place preference. Similar inverted U dose-effect functions have been reported for conditioned place preference with other psychostimulants such as amphetamine, cocaine, and methamphetamine (Adriani and Laviola, 2003;RodriguezAlarcòn et al, 2007;Zakharova et al, 2009). Other cathinones also produce reward and reinforcement.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 66%
“…The doseeffect curves for a-PBP and a-PVP were both inverted U-shaped functions, with the low and high doses not producing conditioned place preference. Similar inverted U dose-effect functions have been reported for conditioned place preference with other psychostimulants such as amphetamine, cocaine, and methamphetamine (Adriani and Laviola, 2003;RodriguezAlarcòn et al, 2007;Zakharova et al, 2009). Other cathinones also produce reward and reinforcement.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 66%
“…Although there is evidence for adolescent reduced sensitivity to the reinforcing properties of opiate drugs (Doherty et al 2013;Doherty and Frantz 2012;MayerBlackwell et al 2014;Niikura et al 2013), the existing research on age-dependent differences in the reinforcing and locomotor-activating effects of psychostimulants is equivocal. Studies have shown that adolescents are more sensitive (Zakharova et al 2009;Mathews et al 2011), less sensitive Mathews and McCormick 2007;Zakharova et al 2009), or no different (Mathews and McCormick, 2007) than their adult counterparts. One possibility is that age differences in pharmacokinetics could account for reduced sensitivity to METH in adolescents, but findings are again mixed.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As noted, adolescents display weaker taste avoidance conditioning in general, and this, too, could be a function of poorer learning in this group. Although possible, adolescents actually display greater drug-induced place preferences (see Badanich et al, 2006;Belluzzi et al, 2004;Brenhouse & Andersen, 2008;Douglas et al, 2003;Torres et al, 2008;Vastola et al, 2002;Zakharova et al, 2009), suggesting that learning in general is not impaired in adolescents. While it is possible that there is a selective learning deficit associated with aversive conditioning (including taste avoidance learning), classic fear conditioning work predicated upon contextual learning suggests that age does not influence the acquisition or extinction of conditioning (Denenberg & Kline, 1958;Kirby, 1963), Interestingly, the retention of such avoidance responses does display a U-shaped function of age (Denenberg & Kline, 1958;Kirby, 1963;Campbell & Campbell, 1962), suggesting that retention of such learning is age dependent.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%