2013
DOI: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.03.027
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Differences on morphological and phonological processing between typically developing children and children with Down syndrome

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

2
3
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 44 publications
2
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…These results are in line with Pérez-Pereira (1989), who found that TD children (aged from three to six years) score significantly lower when the -es allomorph is required. The same pattern has been observed in Spanish children with Down Syndrome (Lázaro, Garayzábal, & Moraleda, 2013). Additionally, we found that children with ta b l e 5.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 87%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…These results are in line with Pérez-Pereira (1989), who found that TD children (aged from three to six years) score significantly lower when the -es allomorph is required. The same pattern has been observed in Spanish children with Down Syndrome (Lázaro, Garayzábal, & Moraleda, 2013). Additionally, we found that children with ta b l e 5.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 87%
“…These results are in line Pérez-Pereira ( 1989 ), who found that TD children (aged from three to six years) score signifi cantly lower when the -es allomorph is required. The same has been observed in children with Down Syndrome (Lázaro, Garayzábal, & Moraleda, 2013 better with real nouns. Again, this is also in line with Pérez-Pereira ( 1989 ), who found that TD children have more problems when they have to pluralize pseudo-words.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 83%
“…Some studies have shown that children with and without language difficulties (including children with Down syndrome: Lázaro, Moraleda & Garayzábal, ; and Williams syndrome: Burani et al ., ) benefit from the effect of the frequency of morphemic constituents. When confronted with a complex word that has a high‐frequency base, children with reading deficits performed better on definition tasks (Lázaro, ; Lázaro, Schreuder & Aceituno, ) and more quickly and accurately on lexical decision and naming tasks (Carlisle & Katz, ; Lázaro et al ., ; Traficante, Marelli, Luzzatti & Burani, ; Verhoeven & Schreuder, ) than when they processed complex words with a low‐frequency base.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…At about two years of age, these children understand morphosyntactic plural markers (Arias-Trejo, Cantrell, Smith, & Alva Canto, 2014). However, Spanishspeaking children with Down syndrome (DS) have difficulty in producing these markers (Lazaro, Garayzabal, & Moraleda, 2013), and there is indirect evidence suggesting that their comprehension of plural morphology might be impaired: English-speaking children with DS have difficulty comprehending number morphology (Joffe & Varlokosta, 2007). Traditionally, this difficulty has been attributed to the lack of general morphological skills found in children with DS (Abbeduto, Warren, & Conners, 2007;Chapman, Schwartz, & Bird, 1991).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Little is known about the mastery of Spanish plural morphosyntax by children with DS. Spanish-speaking children with DS have difficulty pluralizing nouns in a Berko-like production task, and, like typically developing children (Arias-Trejo, Abreu-Mendoza et al, 2014;Pérez-Pereira, 1989), they have more difficulty with words requiring the allomorph /-es/ than with those that require /-s/, the predominant Spanish grammatical plural marker (Lazaro et al, 2013). To date, there are no studies investigating the comprehension of Spanish plural markers in children with DS.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%