2006
DOI: 10.1167/6.10.6
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Depth of interocular suppression associated with continuous flash suppression, flash suppression, and binocular rivalry

Abstract: When conflicting images are presented to the corresponding regions of the two eyes, only one image may be consciously perceived. In binocular rivalry (BR), two images alternate in phenomenal visibility; even a salient image is eventually suppressed by an image of low saliency. Recently, N. Tsuchiya and C. Koch (2005) reported a technique called continuous flash suppression (CFS), extending the suppression duration more than 10-fold. Here, we investigated the depth of this prolonged form of interocular suppress… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

9
191
1

Year Published

2008
2008
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
8
2

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 191 publications
(211 citation statements)
references
References 64 publications
9
191
1
Order By: Relevance
“…(iii) Whereas endogenous attention has some effect on the dominant stimulus, exogenous attention (attention ''grabbing") in the suppressed stimulus will bring it out of suppression (Fox & Check, 1968). Attention grapping thus decreases the dominance period of the other stimulus and is explained by an increase in strength of the error signal (this seems consistent with findings on continuous flash suppression; Tsuchiya, Koch, Gilroy, & Blake, 2006).…”
Section: Modulation Of Dominance Durationsupporting
confidence: 69%
“…(iii) Whereas endogenous attention has some effect on the dominant stimulus, exogenous attention (attention ''grabbing") in the suppressed stimulus will bring it out of suppression (Fox & Check, 1968). Attention grapping thus decreases the dominance period of the other stimulus and is explained by an increase in strength of the error signal (this seems consistent with findings on continuous flash suppression; Tsuchiya, Koch, Gilroy, & Blake, 2006).…”
Section: Modulation Of Dominance Durationsupporting
confidence: 69%
“…Models of suppressive contrast gain control have prompted a resurgence of interest in ocular interactions in psychophysics (Meese & Hess, 2004;Maehara & Goryo, 2005;Ding & Sperling, 2006;Meese et al, 2006;Tsuchiya et al, 2006;Medina et al, 2007;Baker et al, 2007aBaker et al, , 2007bWeiler et al, 2007), electrophysiology (Walker et al 1998;Truchard et al, 2000;Li et al, 2005;Sengpiel & Vorobyov, 2005), and functional imaging (BĂŒchert et al, 2002). These studies have driven the development of binocular models of masking, where interocular suppression forms part of the divisive contrast gain control (Walker et al, 1998;Meese & Hess, 2004;Maehara & Goryo, 2005;Meese et al, 2006;Baker et al, 2007a).…”
Section: Gain Control and Ocular Interactionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In CFS, a stream of salient patterns flashed to the dominant eye renders the signal presented to the non-dominant eye invisible (Tsuchiya & Koch, 2005;Tsuchiya, Koch, Gilroy, & Blake, 2006). Relying on previous results (Anstis, Giaschi, & Cogan, 1985;Blake, Ahlström, & Alais, 1999;Geisler, 1999;Jordan, Fallah, & Stoner, 2006;Kruse, Stadler, & Wehner, 1986;Rajimehr, Vaziri-Pashkam, Afraz, & Esteky, 2004;Ramachandran, 1975;Troje, Sadr, Geyer, & Nakayama, 2006;Wiesenfelder & Blake, 1991), we assumed that in the presence of temporal integration, being exposed to apparent motion with a particular direction or a point-light walker with kinematic features (hereafter the adaptor) would bias the way subsequent ambiguous motion is perceived (i.e., apparent motion with ambiguous direction, or point-light walkers with ambiguous gender).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%