2011
DOI: 10.3758/s13414-010-0073-7
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Crossmodal correspondences: A tutorial review

Abstract: In many everyday situations, our senses are bombarded by many different unisensory signals at any given time. To gain the most veridical, and least variable, estimate of environmental stimuli/properties, we need to combine the individual noisy unisensory perceptual estimates that refer to the same object, while keeping those estimates belonging to different objects or events separate. How, though, does the brain "know" which stimuli to combine? Traditionally, researchers interested in the crossmodal binding pr… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

37
1,198
3
7

Year Published

2013
2013
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 1,147 publications
(1,245 citation statements)
references
References 209 publications
37
1,198
3
7
Order By: Relevance
“…While the bouba-kiki effect is detected in infants around 11-adults (e.g., Maurer et al, 2006;Imai et al, 2008;Imai & Kita, 2014;Spence, 2011;etc. ), young infants only show sensitivities to arbitrary speech-sound correspondences before 6 months of age under specific circumstances (Experiment 1 of this study; Fort et al, 2013;Ozturk et al, 2013 when using non-words with syllable repetitions).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…While the bouba-kiki effect is detected in infants around 11-adults (e.g., Maurer et al, 2006;Imai et al, 2008;Imai & Kita, 2014;Spence, 2011;etc. ), young infants only show sensitivities to arbitrary speech-sound correspondences before 6 months of age under specific circumstances (Experiment 1 of this study; Fort et al, 2013;Ozturk et al, 2013 when using non-words with syllable repetitions).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is established that children and adults are sensitive to non-arbitrary crossmodal correspondences (for review, see Spence, 2011). A well-known example of this research is the tendency in adults and children to associate rounded shapes with specific types of speech sound combinations (e.g., "bouba"), but angular shapes with other types of speech sounds (e.g., "kiki").…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…That is, motion paired with emitted sounds may satisfy the input conditions of adaptations for picking up on invariant sound/object co-relations, irrespective of whether the object is a living thing vocalizing, or a non-living object producing noise [27][28][29][30][31][32][33][34]54]. However, because the acoustics produced by the living and non-living worlds are interestingly different from one another, both in terms of their properties and also in their affordances [9,16,22,23], the psychological mechanisms for representing and reasoning about the living versus the non-living worlds will likely become increasingly distinct from one another over the course of development.…”
Section: (A) Implicationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There are several studies regarding cross-modal correspondences in perception, and Spence's comprehensive tutorial highlights some of them (Spence, 2011). For example, high-pitched sounds are usually related to small bright lights and to higher spatial positioning, whereas slow movement is associated to darker ambiances, long and low pitched sounds.…”
Section: Vocabularymentioning
confidence: 99%