1920
DOI: 10.1037/h0069608
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Conditioned emotional reactions.

Abstract: speculations havebeen entered intoconcerning possibility of conditioning various typesof emotionalresponse. but diexperimental evidence in support of such a view has been lacking. If the advanced by Warson and Morgan to the effect that in infancy the emotionalreaction patterns are few, consisting. so far as observed of and love, then there must be some simple method hy means of range of stimuliwhichcancallout these emotions and theircomis greatly increased. Otherwise, complexity in adultresponse could authors … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

2
445
3
21

Year Published

1932
1932
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 1,559 publications
(486 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
2
445
3
21
Order By: Relevance
“…These studies often pointed toward the amygdala (Morris et al, 1998;Critchley et al, 2002;Knight et al, 2009;Tabbert et al, 2011) as the neural substrate of Pavlovian fear learning, but also received substantial criticism based on methodological (potential residual CS awareness; Mitchell et al, 2009) and statistical (Vadillo et al, 2016) grounds. Even evidence for fear conditioning in nonverbal human children (Watson and Rayner, 1920) still leaves open the main question that motivated our research: once a human becomes verbal, can these verbal processes override learning pathways via experience, or do we keep separable pathways for experience-based fear conditioning instead? In other words, it remains unclear whether the human brain reserves space for the unique impact of actually experiencing CS-US pairings, in the face of explicit fear instructions.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These studies often pointed toward the amygdala (Morris et al, 1998;Critchley et al, 2002;Knight et al, 2009;Tabbert et al, 2011) as the neural substrate of Pavlovian fear learning, but also received substantial criticism based on methodological (potential residual CS awareness; Mitchell et al, 2009) and statistical (Vadillo et al, 2016) grounds. Even evidence for fear conditioning in nonverbal human children (Watson and Rayner, 1920) still leaves open the main question that motivated our research: once a human becomes verbal, can these verbal processes override learning pathways via experience, or do we keep separable pathways for experience-based fear conditioning instead? In other words, it remains unclear whether the human brain reserves space for the unique impact of actually experiencing CS-US pairings, in the face of explicit fear instructions.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Bekhterev, 1913;Kim & Jung, 2006;Pape & Pare, 2010;Pape & Stork, 2003), and has also long been proven effective in humans (e.g. Fendt & Fanselow, 1999;LaBar, Gatenby, Gore, LeDoux, & Phelps, 1998;Sehlmeyer et al, 2009;Watson & Rayner, 1920). In a classic fear-conditioning paradigm, an initially neutral stimulus (CS) such as a sound is associated with an aversive unconditioned stimulus (US) such as an electrical shock, while another neutral stimulus remains unpaired (CS−) (Maren, 2001;Pavlov, 1927).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The traditional behavioral approach to the study of irrational fears and their treatment drew heavily on respondent conditioning and extinction (Rachman, 1977;Watson & Rayner, 1920). The basic model involved a conditioning history in which a neutral stimulus (CS) was directly paired with an unconditioned stimulus (UCS) that evoked a fear response.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%