2014
DOI: 10.1016/j.ridd.2014.07.055
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Concrete and relational vocabulary: Comparison between Williams and Smith–Magenis syndromes

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

1
5
0
1

Year Published

2016
2016
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
1
5
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…This contrast demonstrates that despite a relative strength in verbal ability in WS, this does not encompass spatial language ability. This is consistent with previous assessments of spatial language in WS (Phillips et al, 2004; Landau and Hoffman, 2005; Laing and Jarrold, 2007; Heinze et al, 2014). This impairment demonstrates a cross-domain interaction; spatial language ability, although part of the verbal domain, is more in line with spatial cognition than verbal cognition in WS.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This contrast demonstrates that despite a relative strength in verbal ability in WS, this does not encompass spatial language ability. This is consistent with previous assessments of spatial language in WS (Phillips et al, 2004; Landau and Hoffman, 2005; Laing and Jarrold, 2007; Heinze et al, 2014). This impairment demonstrates a cross-domain interaction; spatial language ability, although part of the verbal domain, is more in line with spatial cognition than verbal cognition in WS.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…Phillips et al (2004) found that individuals with WS struggled to understand these spatial terms, relative to TD controls and individuals with moderate learning difficulties of the same verbal MA. Relatedly, Heinze et al (2014) demonstrated that spatial relational vocabulary was marginally weaker than concrete vocabulary in WS. These findings were further supported by Laing and Jarrold (2007) who demonstrated that individuals with WS find it difficult to create a mental model of spatial language terms.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 91%
“…Morphosyntactic abilities had been considered selectively spared ( Clahsen et al, 2004 ), although this assumption was challenged in several studies indicating some degree of atypical morphological processing ( Thomas et al, 2001 ; Boloh and Ibernon, 2010 ; Benítez-Burraco et al, 2017 ; Diez-Itza et al, 2017 ). Receptive vocabulary is also an area of relative strength in people with Down syndrome, but only for concrete vocabulary ( Mervis and John, 2008 ; Garayzábal et al, 2014 ; Moraleda and López, 2020 ). Regarding lexical production, a tendency to use rare words and an atypical pattern of semantic categorization has been reported ( Bellugi et al, 1994 ; Purser et al, 2010 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Neurodevelopmental disorders are related to a specific cognitive profile, which is characterized by moderate intellectual disability , poor working memory, and attention deficit and hyperactivity. Language is delayed and speech disorders continue into adulthood (Udwin et al 2001;Solomon et al 2002;Gropman et al 2006;Garayzábal Heinze et al 2011;Osório et al 2012;Heinze and Lens 2013;Garayzábal et al 2014;Hidalgo and Garayzábal 2019). Concerning the behavioral phenotype, individuals with SMS tend to easily show states of anxiety and nervousness, especially in new situations.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%