2012
DOI: 10.1007/s00221-012-3316-0
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Cognitive mapping in humans and its relationship to other orientation skills

Abstract: Human orientation in novel and familiar environments is a complex skill that can involve numerous different strategies. To date, a comprehensive account of how these strategies interrelate at the behavioural level has not been documented, impeding the development of elaborate systems neuroscience models of spatial orientation. Here, we describe a virtual environment test battery designed to assess five of the core strategies used by humans to orient. Our results indicate that the ability to form a cognitive ma… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

6
42
0
2

Year Published

2014
2014
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 51 publications
(50 citation statements)
references
References 71 publications
6
42
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…The different impact of aging on spatial frames of reference confirms that egocentric and allocentric frames form specialized and interactive functions underpinned, at least partially, by neural areas that are differently vulnerable to normal aging processes (Arnold et al, 2013;Galati et al, 2010;Iachini et al, 2009a, b, c;Montefinese et al, 2014;Ruggiero et al, 2014).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 78%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The different impact of aging on spatial frames of reference confirms that egocentric and allocentric frames form specialized and interactive functions underpinned, at least partially, by neural areas that are differently vulnerable to normal aging processes (Arnold et al, 2013;Galati et al, 2010;Iachini et al, 2009a, b, c;Montefinese et al, 2014;Ruggiero et al, 2014).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 78%
“…Spatial information is stored in human memory according to two classes of spatial frames of reference: egocentric and allocentric (Arnold et al, 2013;Burgess, 2006;Iachini, Ruggiero, Conson, & Trojano, 2009b;O'Keefe & Nadel, 1978;Paillard, 1991;Piaget & Inhelder, 1967;Ruggiero, Frassinetti, Iavarone, & Iachini, 2014; for a review see Galati, Pelle, Berthoz, & Committeri, 2010). Egocentric frames of reference use the organism as the center of the organization of surrounding space.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Most notably, the participants' navigational ability was assessed with a self-reported questionnaire (i.e., SBSOD), and the effect sizes of SBSOD in relation to behavioral performance in virtual and real-world environments appears to be relatively small (Arnold et al 2013;Wolbers and Hegarty 2010). For example, the effect sizes for correlations with SBSOD scores were 0.18 for both the formation and use of cognitive maps (Arnold et al 2013), similar to those observed in the present study, especially for small-worldness (i.e., 0.18). Thus, it is unclear whether these findings of behavioral relevance could translate to behavioral performance in different navigation tasks.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In contrast, most theoretical proposals regarding spatial coding suggest that space involves a holistic, multidimensional representation in which details of the environment become integrated across multiple viewings and time points [5963]. Behavioral studies support this assertion, suggesting that forming a cognitive map involves integrating multiple trajectories through space across time [59,60,64], which creates a more holistic, less elementized representation [59]. Thus, one possible interpretation of our findings is that near and far items differed in their representation for spatial vs. temporal context due to differences in how a temporal vs. spatial layout is stored associatively.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%