2000
DOI: 10.1901/jeab.2000.73-79
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Choice Between Constant and Variable Alternatives by Rats: Effects of Different Reinforcer Amounts and Energy Budgets

Abstract: Two experiments, using rats as subjects, investigated the effect of different reinforcer amounts and energy budgets on choice between constant and variable alternatives under a closed economy. Rats were housed in the chamber and were exposed to a modified concurrent-chains schedule in which the choice phase was separated from a rest phase during which the rats could engage in other activities. In the choice phase, a single variable-interval schedule arranged entry into one of two equal terminal links (fixed-in… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
12
0
1

Year Published

2003
2003
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 22 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 36 publications
0
12
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The VA distributions used in the current experiment were positive and negative exponential and rectangular, providing a minimum of zero and a maximum of 12 tokens. It has been noted that exponential distribution types (negative binomial in particular) represent food sources that many species experience in their natural foraging environments (Ito, Takatsuru, & Saeki, 2000; Iwasa, Higashi, & Yamamura, 1981). Because a range of distribution types have been employed in risky choice studies, such as bimodal (two‐valued; e.g., Caraco, Martindale, & Whittam, 1980; Hamm & Shettleworth, 1987; Leventhal, Morrell, Morgan, & Perkins, 1959), it is not entirely clear how differently programmed variability may affect risk‐sensitive preferences.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The VA distributions used in the current experiment were positive and negative exponential and rectangular, providing a minimum of zero and a maximum of 12 tokens. It has been noted that exponential distribution types (negative binomial in particular) represent food sources that many species experience in their natural foraging environments (Ito, Takatsuru, & Saeki, 2000; Iwasa, Higashi, & Yamamura, 1981). Because a range of distribution types have been employed in risky choice studies, such as bimodal (two‐valued; e.g., Caraco, Martindale, & Whittam, 1980; Hamm & Shettleworth, 1987; Leventhal, Morrell, Morgan, & Perkins, 1959), it is not entirely clear how differently programmed variability may affect risk‐sensitive preferences.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These particular schedules were chosen in order to provide a wide range of rates of reinforcement and to exceed the range that had been used in other studies to take animals from a positive to a negative energy budget (e.g., Ito et al, 2000). They were FI and VI 15-sec, FI and VI 105sec, FI and VI 45-sec, FI and VI 5-sec, and FI and VI 225-sec schedules in different conditions, conducted in that order.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Positive energy budgets provide enough energy for the animal to meet its fitness needs. As a result, larger animals, such as pigeons and rats, have been used to test RST (e.g., Ito, Takatsuru, & Saeki, 2000). Early versions of RST defined fitness solely in terms of survival.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, when presented with two sources of reinforcement, animals allocate their behavior to match the ratio of reinforcement schedules [10,11], reinforcer amounts [12,13], reinforcer immediacies [14,15], and the relative rate of conditioned reinforcers [16]. Although the factors determining steady-state preference are well described [17], the factors controlling how preference adapts when reinforcer properties change are poorly understood.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%