1991
DOI: 10.2307/2982696
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Can Jurors Understand Probabilistic Evidence?

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
23
0

Year Published

2000
2000
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
5
5

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 90 publications
(26 citation statements)
references
References 12 publications
3
23
0
Order By: Relevance
“…As SIPS scores change gradually, P(M jT i ) values change gradually; Finally, Dr. B's ®ctional SIPS results show that developing a multiple-cut-off interpretation scheme for a malingering measure would be as feasible as was the development of the SIRS, and would not require the``monumental effort'' of examining thousands of subjects, as Rogers and Salekin (1998, p. 153) (Smith, Penrod, Otto, & Park, 1998) suggests, among other things, that jurors may underutilize Bayesian probabilities, a ®nding that accords with previous research (Kaye & Koehler, 1991), and that should assuage some commentators' (e.g., Tribe, 1971) and courts' concerns that frequency statistics will have an overwhelming``impact on the trier of fact'' (State v. Boyd, 1983, p. 482). The issue of how well persons use base rates and probabilistic information is highly complicated and a source of contention among scholars (Continuing Commentary, 1997;Koehler, 1996).…”
supporting
confidence: 62%
“…As SIPS scores change gradually, P(M jT i ) values change gradually; Finally, Dr. B's ®ctional SIPS results show that developing a multiple-cut-off interpretation scheme for a malingering measure would be as feasible as was the development of the SIRS, and would not require the``monumental effort'' of examining thousands of subjects, as Rogers and Salekin (1998, p. 153) (Smith, Penrod, Otto, & Park, 1998) suggests, among other things, that jurors may underutilize Bayesian probabilities, a ®nding that accords with previous research (Kaye & Koehler, 1991), and that should assuage some commentators' (e.g., Tribe, 1971) and courts' concerns that frequency statistics will have an overwhelming``impact on the trier of fact'' (State v. Boyd, 1983, p. 482). The issue of how well persons use base rates and probabilistic information is highly complicated and a source of contention among scholars (Continuing Commentary, 1997;Koehler, 1996).…”
supporting
confidence: 62%
“…Research has generally found that simulated jurors are less responsive to forensic evidence than Bayesian models indicate they should be (Thompson & Schumann, 1987;Faigman & Baglioni, 1988;Goodman, 1992;Smith et al, 1996;Schklar & Diamond, 1999;Nance & Morris, 2002, 2005Martire et al, 2013Martire et al, , 2014; for reviews of the early studies see Koehler, 2001;Kaye & Koehler, 1991;Thompson, 1989). But Thompson, Kaasa, and Peterson (2013) recently questioned whether jurors always underutilize forensic evidence relative to Bayesian norms.…”
Section: Logical Coherencementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Instead, the evidence suggests that people have a generally poor understanding of probabilities and statistics; tend to confuse likelihoods and posterior beliefs (as in the prosecutor and defence attorney fallacies) 20 ; and tend toward an under-valuing of the evidence as compared to normative (i.e. Bayesian) models [21][22][23][24] . Moreover, meta-analyses of risk communication literature 25 in addition to numerous studies focusing specifically on probabilistic expressions in court [26][27][28][29] suggest that decision-makers are sensitive to the form in which statistics are presented and have a preference for natural frequencies or event rates.…”
Section: Probabilistic Information and Belief Updatingmentioning
confidence: 99%