2021
DOI: 10.1007/s00221-021-06066-z
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Assessing the relative contribution of vision to odometry via manipulations of gait in an over-ground homing task

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

1
7
0

Year Published

2022
2022
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 59 publications
1
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Fragaszy and Mangalam (2018) stress the need for immediate contact with grasping skin or target surface, but cascade-driven interactions allow no straightforward segregation of short-range interaction from more distal forces. Rather than an empty expanse, environmental space is full of clutter whose integral flow supports organisms via contact through distributed-but-rarely-distinct modalities (e.g., optical, acoustic, and haptic (1979), Pittenger (1995), andHarrison et al (2021), respectively). The specificity of localized sensor cells is physiologically real, but they distract from the non-exclusivity and often mutuality of manipulation and, say, observation when mechanical contact is lost (Stoffregen et al, 2017).…”
Section: Overviewmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Fragaszy and Mangalam (2018) stress the need for immediate contact with grasping skin or target surface, but cascade-driven interactions allow no straightforward segregation of short-range interaction from more distal forces. Rather than an empty expanse, environmental space is full of clutter whose integral flow supports organisms via contact through distributed-but-rarely-distinct modalities (e.g., optical, acoustic, and haptic (1979), Pittenger (1995), andHarrison et al (2021), respectively). The specificity of localized sensor cells is physiologically real, but they distract from the non-exclusivity and often mutuality of manipulation and, say, observation when mechanical contact is lost (Stoffregen et al, 2017).…”
Section: Overviewmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…When actively traversing (e.g., walking through) an environment, multiple sources of information about self-motion are redundantly available. This includes visual information (Ellmore & McNaughton, 2004), vestibular information (Israel et al, 1997;Mittelstaedt, 1999), and podokinetic information (Glasauer et al 1994(Glasauer et al , 2002Harrison et al, 2021). Podokinetic information refers to the information that is created by the movements of the legs during locomotion (Weber et al, 1998).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Running Head: PODOKINETIC PATH INTEGRATION FOR CRAWLING GAITS 4 Path integration based upon podokinetic information has been investigated in a set of gait manipulation experiments in which direct-route homing tasks have been performed without vision (e.g., while blindfolded) and the gait patterns and gait parameters used in the outbound and inbound phases of the task have been systematically manipulated (Abdolvah et al, 2015;Harrison, 2020;Harrison et al, 2021;Isenhower et al, 2012;Schwartz, 1999;Turvey et al, 2009;. These gait manipulation experiments have repeatedly shown that the podokinetic information supporting path integration is not associated with simple lower-order variables, such as the number of steps, elapsed time, or the energy expended during outbound and inbound travel.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Successful navigation through an environment requires the accurate estimation of one’s traveled distance (path integration) and direction of self-motion (heading). Previous studies have documented the role of visual ( 1 ), vestibular ( 2 ), auditory ( 3 ), and tactile ( 4 , 5 ) information for the processing of traveled distance. Studies on heading perception have mainly focused on visual (e.g., Ref.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%