2018
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02361
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Aging Impairs Disengagement From Negative Words in a Dot Probe Task

Abstract: Age differences in emotional processes have been of great interest. Previous studies using the dot probe task show that older adults can be more influenced by negative emotionally valenced faces than younger adults. Subsequent work has demonstrated two distinctive ways people engage with stimuli in this task, namely orienting to and disengaging from emotional stimuli. In the present study, we examined the effects of aging as well as ability to orient to and disengage from emotional words in a dot probe task. O… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
6
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 28 publications
0
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The deletion function is responsible for clearing the mental workspace to focus only on current concerns by suppressing mental representations that were never relevant in the first instance as well as those that became irrelevant as goals, tasks, and topics changed (e.g., Lustig, May, & Hasher, 2001; Radvansky & Copeland, 2006). Deletion (which has also been referred to as “disengagement” and working memory “updating”; Miyake et al, 2000; Talbot, Ksander, & Gutchess, 2018) also plays a role in successive tasks such as in multiple list recall, in reading comprehension, and in prospective and retrospective memory tasks (Campbell, Trelle, & Hasher, 2014; Hamm & Hasher, 1992; Scullin, Bugg, McDaniel, & Einstein, 2011). Finally, the restraint function allows for selection among competing responses by suppressing prepotent (or relatively automatic) responses in both thought and action (e.g., Spieler, Balota, & Faust, 1996; Williams, Ponesse, Schachar, Logan, & Tannock, 1999).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The deletion function is responsible for clearing the mental workspace to focus only on current concerns by suppressing mental representations that were never relevant in the first instance as well as those that became irrelevant as goals, tasks, and topics changed (e.g., Lustig, May, & Hasher, 2001; Radvansky & Copeland, 2006). Deletion (which has also been referred to as “disengagement” and working memory “updating”; Miyake et al, 2000; Talbot, Ksander, & Gutchess, 2018) also plays a role in successive tasks such as in multiple list recall, in reading comprehension, and in prospective and retrospective memory tasks (Campbell, Trelle, & Hasher, 2014; Hamm & Hasher, 1992; Scullin, Bugg, McDaniel, & Einstein, 2011). Finally, the restraint function allows for selection among competing responses by suppressing prepotent (or relatively automatic) responses in both thought and action (e.g., Spieler, Balota, & Faust, 1996; Williams, Ponesse, Schachar, Logan, & Tannock, 1999).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Though the positivity effect has been documented using the dot-probe task in humans (Lee & Knight, 2009;Orgeta, 2011;Talbot et al, 2018;Tomaszczyk & Fernandes, 2014), the effect only emerges under certain experimental configurations making it altogether not particularly surprising that we did not find effects in this sample. For example, Orgeta (2011) found no evidence of the effect when stimuli were presented for 17 ms or 500 ms but did when stimuli were presented for 1000 ms.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 46%
“…1 For example, compared to younger adults, older adults tend to recall proportionately more positive relative to negative information (Charles et al, 2003;Gerhardsson et al, 2019;Joubert et al, 2018; Q. Kennedy et al, 2004;Mammarella et al, 2016), use more positive language (Kyröläinen et al, 2021), and attend more to positively and less to negatively valenced visual stimuli (Isaacowitz et al, 2006a(Isaacowitz et al, , 2006bLee & Knight, 2009;, 2005Nikitin & Freund, 2011;Orgeta, 2011;Talbot et 1 Importantly, the age-related positivity effect does not refer to any age-related biases in affective experience. While there is some evidence of improvement in affective experiences with age (Carstensen et al, 2011;Mroczek, 2001;Sheibe et al, 2013), the positivity effect is solely concerned with information processing (e.g., memory and attention) biases related to valenced stimuli.…”
Section: See No Evil: Attentional Bias Towards Threat Is Diminished I...mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Though the positivity effect has been documented using the dot-probe task in humans (L. O. Lee & Knight, 2009; Mather & Carstensen, 2003; Orgeta, 2011; Talbot et al, 2018; Tomaszczyk & Fernandes, 2014), the effect only emerges under certain experimental configurations making it altogether not particularly surprising that we did not find effects in this sample. For example, Orgeta (2011) found no evidence of the effect when stimuli were presented for 17 or 500 ms but did when stimuli were presented for 1,000 ms.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 48%