2009
DOI: 10.1590/s0004-282x2009000300005
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A kinesthetic motor imagery study in patients with writer' cramp

Abstract: -The aim was to determine if patients with writer's cramp (WC) have abnormalities in kinesthetic motor imagery of hand movements. We timed the execution and simulation of a "finger tap task" and a "writing task" in 9 patients with simple WC and 9 matched healthy controls. In the "finger tap task, patients tended to be slower than controls to execute without vision (p=0.190) and to simulate the movements (p=0.094). In the "writing task", patients were slower than controls to execute writing with vision (p=0.000… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 8 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 28 publications
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…With regards to explicit motor imagery, patients with focal hand dystonia appear to be slower than healthy controls during the imagination of writing and tapping movements (Tumas and Sakamoto, 2009).…”
Section: Cognitive Representation Of Movementmentioning
confidence: 96%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…With regards to explicit motor imagery, patients with focal hand dystonia appear to be slower than healthy controls during the imagination of writing and tapping movements (Tumas and Sakamoto, 2009).…”
Section: Cognitive Representation Of Movementmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…Different paradigms have been used to study cognitive representation of movement, such as explicit motor imagery (Delnooz et al, 2013;Delnooz et al, 2012;Tumas and Sakamoto, 2009), mental rotation of body parts Fiorio et al, 2006; and temporal expectation of movements outcome (Table. 1).…”
Section: Cognitive Representation Of Movementmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Motor imagery often accurately simulates the time taken to execute actions across a variety of tasks, including pointing (Decety & Michel, 1989; Watson & Rubin, 1996), writing (Papaxanthis, Pozzo, Skoura, & Schieppati, 2002; Tumas & Sakamoato, 2009), locomotion (Saimpont, Malouin, Tousignant, & Jackson, 2012) and sporting activities (MacIntyre & Moran, 1996; Oishi, Kasai, & Maeshima, 2000). Motor imagery and overt actions show similar effects of various cognitive variables such as word labels and visual illusions (Glover, Dixon, Castiello, & Rushworth, 2005; Glover & Dixon, 2013), and both generally adhere to Fitts’ Law (Cerritelli, Maruff, Wilson, & Currie, 2000; Decety & Jeannerod, 1995; Macuga & Frey, 2014; Macuga, Papailiou, & Frey, 2012; Maruff, Wilson, De Fazio, Cerritelli, Hedt, & Currie, 1999; Sirigu et al, 1995; but see Young, Pratt, & Chau, 2008).…”
Section: Behavioral Evidence Favors the Motor-cognitive Modelmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some data described the effects of FHD on motor imagery of different movements. In particular, in order to understand whether the physical impairments due to FHD generally or specifically influence the characteristics of mental imagery, patients suffering from writer’s cramp were asked to physically perform and mentally imagine finger tapping and writing (Tumas and Sakamoto, 2009). Surprisingly, with respect to healthy controls, patients had longer motor imagery latencies for both actions, suggesting that FHD would lead to unspecific deficits in mental imagery of complex movements.…”
Section: Mental Imagery and Rotation As “Clean” Tools To Investigate mentioning
confidence: 99%